請用此 Handle URI 來引用此文件:
http://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/89807
完整後設資料紀錄
DC 欄位 | 值 | 語言 |
---|---|---|
dc.contributor.advisor | 陳顥齡 | zh_TW |
dc.contributor.advisor | Hao-Ling Chen | en |
dc.contributor.author | 洪品蓁 | zh_TW |
dc.contributor.author | Pin-Chen Hung | en |
dc.date.accessioned | 2023-09-22T16:12:08Z | - |
dc.date.available | 2023-11-10 | - |
dc.date.copyright | 2023-09-22 | - |
dc.date.issued | 2023 | - |
dc.date.submitted | 2023-08-11 | - |
dc.identifier.citation | Reference
Adams, A.-M., Simmons, F., & Willis, C. (2015). Exploring relationships between working memory and writing: Individual differences associated with gender. Learning and Individual Differences, 40, 101-107. Amundson, S. J., & Weil, M. (1996). Prewriting and handwriting skills. Occupational therapy for children, 3(1), 524-541. Anderson, R. C., Ku, Y.-M., Li, W., Chen, X., Wu, X., & Shu, H. (2013). Learning to see the patterns in Chinese characters. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(1), 41-56. Baddeley, A. (2010). Working memory. Curr Biol, 20(4), R136-140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.014 Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu Rev Psychol, 63, 1-29. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422 Beery, K. E. (1997). The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration. Administration scoring and teaching manual. Benbow, M., Hanft, B. E., & Marsh, D. (1992). Handwriting in the classroom: Improving written communication. American Occupational Therapy Association. Berninger, V., Yates, C., Cartwright, A., Rutberg, J., Remy, E., & Abbott, R. (1992). Lower-level developmental skills in beginning writing. Reading and Writing, 4(3), 257-280. Bigozzi, L., Malagoli, C., Pecini, C., Pezzica, S., Vezzani, C., & Vettori, G. (2021). Attention Components and Spelling Accuracy: Which Connections Matter? Children (Basel), 8(7). https://doi.org/10.3390/children8070539 Bourke, L., & Adams, A.-M. (2003). The relationship between working memory and early writing assessed at the word, sentence and text level. Educational and Child Psychology, 20, 19-36. Brooks, A. D., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2011). Letter naming and letter writing reversals in children with dyslexia: momentary inefficiency in the phonological and orthographic loops of working memory. Dev Neuropsychol, 36(7), 847-868. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2011.606401 Brown, T., & Link, J. (2016). The association between measures of visual perception, visual-motor integration, and in-hand manipulation skills of school-age children and their manuscript handwriting speed. British Journal of Occupational Therapy, 79(3), 163-171. Brown, T., & Peres, L. (2018). An overview and critique of the Test of Visual Perception Skills–fourth edition (TVPS-4). Hong Kong Journal of Occupational Therapy, 31(2), 59-68. Case, R., Kurland, D. M., & Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of experimental child psychology, 33(3), 386-404. Chang, S.-H., Yu, N.-Y., & Shie, J.-J. (2009). The Preliminary Development of Computer-Assisted Assessment of Chinese Handwriting Performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 108(3), 887-904. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.108.3.887-904 Chang, S. H., & Yu, N. Y. (2005). Evaluation and classification of types of Chinese handwriting deficits in elementary schoolchildren. Percept Mot Skills, 101(2), 631-647. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.101.2.631-647 Chang, S. H., & Yu, N. Y. (2013). Handwriting movement analyses comparing first and second graders with normal or dysgraphic characteristics. Res Dev Disabil, 34(9), 2433-2441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.02.028 Chase, C. I. (1986). Essay Test Scoring: Interaction of Relevant Variables. Journal of Educational Measurement, 23(1), 33-41. http://www.jstor.org/stable/1434642 Cho, J.-R., McBride, C., & Kim, B. (2020). Effective teaching instructions for Hangul learning among Korean kindergartners. Reading and Writing, 33(7), 1791-1808. Cornhill, H., & Case-Smith, J. (1996). Factors that relate to good and poor handwriting. Am J Occup Ther, 50(9), 732-739. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.50.9.732 Costa, V., Fischer-Baum, S., Capasso, R., Miceli, G., & Rapp, B. (2011). Temporal stability and representational distinctiveness: Key functions of orthographic working memory. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 28, 338-362. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2011.648921 Crespo, Y., Soriano, M. F., Iglesias-Parro, S., Aznarte, J. I., & Ibáñez-Molina, A. J. (2018). Spatial Analysis of Handwritten Texts as a Marker of Cognitive Control. J Mot Behav, 50(6), 643-652. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2017.1400945 DeBono, T., Hosseini, A., Cairo, C., Ghelani, K., Tannock, R., & Toplak, M. E. (2012). Written expression performance in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Reading and Writing, 25(6), 1403-1426. Di Brina, C., Niels, R., Overvelde, A., Levi, G., & Hulstijn, W. (2008). Dynamic time warping: a new method in the study of poor handwriting. Hum Mov Sci, 27(2), 242-255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2008.02.012 Exner, C. E. (2001). Development of hand skills. Occupational therapy for children, 5, 304-355. Feder, K. P., & Majnemer, A. (2007). Handwriting development, competency, and intervention. Dev Med Child Neurol, 49(4), 312-317. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00312.x Gargot, T., Asselborn, T., Pellerin, H., Zammouri, I., S, M. A., Casteran, L., Johal, W., Dillenbourg, P., Cohen, D., & Jolly, C. (2020). Acquisition of handwriting in children with and without dysgraphia: A computational approach. PLOS ONE, 15(9), e0237575. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237575 Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental psychology, 40(2), 177. Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Fink, B. (2000). Is handwriting causally related to learning to write? Treatment of handwriting problems in beginning writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 620-633. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.620 Homburg, T. J. (1984). Holistic Evaluation of ESL Compositions: Can It Be Validated Objectively? TESOL Quarterly, 18(1), 87-107. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2307/3586337 Hoy, M. M., Egan, M. Y., & Feder, K. P. (2011). A systematic review of interventions to improve handwriting. Can J Occup Ther, 78(1), 13-25. https://doi.org/10.2182/cjot.2011.78.1.3 Huang, W. F., Wang, T. N., Chuang, P. Y., & Chen, H. L. (2023). Development and Psychometric Evaluation of the Computer-Aided Measure of Chinese Handwriting Legibility (CAM-CHL) for School-Age Children. Am J Occup Ther, 77(1). https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2023.050075 Hung, L., Chang, Y., Chen, S., Chen, C., & Lee, Y. (2003). Battery of Chinese basic literacy (BCBL). Taipei, Taiwan: Psychological Company. Hwang, Y. S., Su, P. F., Hsiao, Y. L., Tsai, W. H., & Hung, J. Y. (2020). Developing a Nomogram Model to Predict the Risk of Poor Chinese Handwriting in First Grade. Am J Occup Ther, 74(5), 7405205080p7405205081-7405205080p7405205088. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2020.038711 Juel, C., Griffith, P. L., & Gough, P. B. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243-255. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.78.4.243 Kandel, S., & Perret, C. (2015). How does the interaction between spelling and motor processes build up during writing acquisition? Cognition, 136, 325-336. Kellogg, R. T. (2008). Training writing skills: A cognitive developmental perspective. Journal of writing research, 1(1), 1-26. Klein, S., Guiltner, V., Sollereder, P., & Cui, Y. (2011). Relationships between fine-motor, visual-motor, and visual perception scores and handwriting legibility and speed. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr, 31(1), 103-114. https://doi.org/10.3109/01942638.2010.541753 Lam, S. S.-Y., & McBride, C. (2018). Learning to write: The role of handwriting for Chinese spelling in kindergarten children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(7), 917. Lam, S. S. T., Au, R. K. C., Leung, H. W. H., & Li-Tsang, C. W. P. (2011). Chinese handwriting performance of primary school children with dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(5), 1745-1756. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.03.001 Lee, H. Y., Chen, R. A., Lin, Y. S., Yang, Y. C., Huang, C. W., & Chen, S. C. (2014). The written language performance of children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Taiwan. Res Dev Disabil, 35(8), 1878-1884. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.04.006 Lee, T. I., Howe, T. H., Chen, H. L., & Wang, T. N. (2016). Predicting Handwriting Legibility in Taiwanese Elementary School Children. Am J Occup Ther, 70(6), 7006220020p7006220021-7006220020p7006220029. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2016.016865 Li-Tsang, C. W., Au, R. K., Chan, M. H., Chan, L. W., Lau, G. M., Lo, T. K., & Leung, H. W. (2011). Handwriting characteristics among secondary students with and without physical disabilities: a study with a computerized tool. Res Dev Disabil, 32(1), 207-216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.09.015 Li, C.-H., Wu, H.-M., Kuo, B.-C., Yang, Y.-M., Lin, C.-K., & Wang, W.-H. (2018). The validity of computerized visual motor integration assessment using Chinese basic strokes. Interactive Learning Environments, 26(8), 1074-1089. Lust, C. A., & Donica, D. K. (2011). Effectiveness of a Handwriting Readiness Program in Head Start: A Two-Group Controlled Trial. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 65(5), 560-568. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2011.000612 Maeland, A. F. (1992). Handwriting and perceptual-motor skills in clumsy, dysgraphic, and ‘normal’children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75(3_suppl), 1207-1217. Marr, D., & Cermak, S. (2002). Predicting handwriting performance of early elementary students with the developmental test of visual-motor integration. Percept Mot Skills, 95(2), 661-669. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.2002.95.2.661 Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (1998). Comparison of scores on two recent editions of the Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87(3_suppl), 1324-1326. Mo, J., McBride, C., & Yip, L. (2018). Identifying the unique role of orthographic working memory in a componential model of Hong Kong kindergarteners’ Chinese written spelling. Reading and Writing, 31(5), 1083-1108. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9829-6 Pak, A. K., Cheng-Lai, A., Tso, I. F., Shu, H., Li, W., & Anderson, R. C. (2005). Visual chunking skills of Hong Kong children. Reading and Writing, 18(5), 437-454. Pfeiffer, B., Rai, G., Murray, T., & Brusilovskiy, E. (2015). Effectiveness of the Size Matters Handwriting Program. OTJR (Thorofare N J), 35(2), 110-119. https://doi.org/10.1177/1539449215573004 Pickering, S. J., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). The Working Memory Test Battery for Children. Re, A. M., Mirandola, C., Esposito, S. S., & Capodieci, A. (2014). Spelling errors among children with ADHD symptoms: the role of working memory. Res Dev Disabil, 35(9), 2199-2204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.05.010 Roman, A. S., Pisoni, D. B., & Kronenberger, W. G. (2014). Assessment of working memory capacity in preschool children using the missing scan task. Infant and child development, 23(6), 575-587. Rosenblum, S., Goldstand, S., & Parush, S. (2006). Relationships among biomechanical ergonomic factors, handwriting product quality, handwriting efficiency, and computerized handwriting process measures in children with and without handwriting difficulties. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 60(1), 28-39. Rosenblum, S., Weiss, P. L., & Parush, S. (2004). Handwriting evaluation for developmental dysgraphia: Process versus product. Reading and Writing, 17(5), 433-458. Sanghavi, R., & Kelkar, R. (2005). Visual-motor integration and learning disabled children. The indian journal of occupational therapy, 37(2), 33-38. Schneck, C. M. (2013). Visual perception. Occupational Therapy for Children. sixth ed. Mosby Inc, 373-403. Seo, S.-M. (2018). The effect of fine motor skills on handwriting legibility in preschool age children. Journal of physical therapy science, 30(2), 324-327. Swanson, H. L., & Berninger, V. W. (1996). Individual differences in children's working memory and writing skill. Journal of experimental child psychology, 63(2), 358-385. Thompson J., McLaughlin T., & M., D. K. (2012). Using tracing and modeling with a handwriting without tears® worksheet to increase handwriting legibility for two preschool students with developmental delays: A brief report. Academic Research International, 2(2), 309. Tindle, R., & Longstaff, M. (2021). Working memory and handwriting and share a common resource: An investigation of shared attention. Current Psychology, 1-12. Towse, J. N., & Hitch, G. (1995). Is there a relationship between task demand and storage space in tests of working memory capacity? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48(1), 108-124. Tse, L. F., Thanapalan, K. C., & Chan, C. C. (2014). Visual-perceptual-kinesthetic inputs on influencing writing performances in children with handwriting difficulties. Res Dev Disabil, 35(2), 340-347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.11.013 Tseng, M. H., & Chow, S. M. (2000). Perceptual-motor function of school-age children with slow handwriting speed. Am J Occup Ther, 54(1), 83-88. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.54.1.83 Tseng, M. H., & Murray, E. A. ( 1994). Differences in Perceptual Motor Measures in Children with Good and Poor Handwriting. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 14, 19-36. Ullrich, L., & Roeltgen, D. (2012). Agraphia. Clinical neuropsychology, 130-151. van Hartingsveldt, M. J., Cup, E. H., Hendriks, J. C., de Vries, L., de Groot, I. J., & Nijhuis-van der Sanden, M. W. (2015). Predictive validity of kindergarten assessments on handwriting readiness. Res Dev Disabil, 36c, 114-124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.08.014 van Hartingsveldt, M. J., De Groot, I. J., Aarts, P. B., & Nijhuis-Van Der Sanden, M. W. (2011). Standardized tests of handwriting readiness: a systematic review of the literature. Dev Med Child Neurol, 53(6), 506-515. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03895.x Volman, M. J., van Schendel, B. M., & Jongmans, M. J. (2006). Handwriting difficulties in primary school children: a search for underlying mechanisms. Am J Occup Ther, 60(4), 451-460. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.60.4.451 Wang, Y., McBride-Chang, C., & Chan, S. F. (2014). Correlates of Chinese kindergarteners’ word reading and writing: The unique role of copying skills? Reading and Writing, 27(7), 1281-1302. Wang, Y., McBride, C., Zhou, Y., Malatesha Joshi, R., & Farver, J. A. M. (2018). Becoming literate in Chinese: A comparison of native‐speaking and non‐native‐speaking children. Journal of Research in Reading, 41(3), 511-524. Yeung, P.-s., Ho, C. S.-h., Chan, D. W.-o., & Chung, K. K.-h. (2016). Orthographic skills important to Chinese literacy development: The role of radical representation and orthographic memory of radicals. Reading and Writing, 29(9), 1935-1958. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9659-3 張韶霞, & 余南瑩. (2010). 有無發展協調障礙之寫字困難兒童的寫字表現與原因之探討 [Analysis of Handwriting Deficits for the Comparison of Children With/Without Developmental Coordination Disorder]. 職能治療學會雜誌, 28(1), 13-28. https://doi.org/10.6594/jtota.2010.28(1).02 陳彙瑜, & 林巾凱. (2020). 發展遲緩幼兒視動整合、精細動作與書寫先備能力相關之研究 [A Research on the Relationship between Visual Motor Integration, Fine Motor and Pre-handwriting For the Kindergarten With Developmental Delay]. 障礙者理解學刊, 17(1), 1-23. https://doi.org/10.6513/juid.202001_17(1).0001 曾美惠. (1993). Factorial Validity of the Tseng Handwriting Problem Checklist [曾氏寫字問題檢核表之因素效度]. 職能治療學會雜誌, 11, 13-27. https://doi.org/10.6594/jtota.1993.11.03 王惠君(2003)。部件識字策略對國小學習障礙學生識字成效之研究。﹝碩士論文。國立彰化師範大學﹞臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統。 | - |
dc.identifier.uri | http://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/89807 | - |
dc.description.abstract | 背景:書寫為學齡孩童重要的職能,尤其對書寫初學階段的小一生更是不容忽視。及早在幼兒園階段發現會影響孩童未來書寫表現的因子並針對其提供適當的訓練與策略顯得極為重要。目前少有追蹤型研究探討幼兒園孩童的書寫前技巧,對其小一書寫表現的影響;另外,也鮮少研究全面考量對小一孩童重要的書寫任務及不同任務對書寫表現的影響。因此,本研究目的為探討幼兒園孩童的書寫前技巧:精細動作、視知覺、視動整合及工作記憶對其小一不同書寫任務表現之影響。
方法:本研究共招募30位大班孩童,在幼兒園時期評估書寫前技巧;小一時期評估其書寫表現。書寫前技巧以本團隊所開發之平板電腦軌跡描繪測驗評估孩童精細動作;視知覺發展測驗第四版評估視知覺;拜瑞-布坦尼卡視覺-動作統整發展測驗第四版評估視動整合以及兩種工作記憶能力測驗;而孩童需完成四種抄寫任務及基本讀寫字綜合測驗作為其書寫表現。透過階層回歸分析探索小一孩童書寫表現的顯著預測因子。 結果及結論:研究結果發現幼兒園時期之視知覺可以有效預測基本讀寫字綜合測驗之表現,工作記憶也為顯著預測因子,並在加入模型後有助於解釋力提升。而視知覺及視動整合可以有效預測其小一近抄中文易讀性。在四種抄寫任務中,視知覺能力在延遲抄寫正確率中扮演重要角色;而不熟悉字抄寫的正確性及四種抄寫任務的筆畫停頓時間則深受工作記憶的影響。未來針對幼兒園孩童可以提供相關預測因子之介入或教學,以達到預防書寫困難之效果。並且可以多加留意工作記憶對整體書寫表現、抄寫時動作停頓所帶來的影響。 | zh_TW |
dc.description.abstract | Background: Handwriting is a crucial occupation for school-age children, especially for first graders who are at the beginning of writing. Early intervention and strategies are important for those who are at risk of handwriting deficiency. Currently, there are few longitudinal studies investigating the impact of handwriting prerequisites in preschoolers on their writing performance in the first grade. Additionally, there is limited research that comprehensively considers writing tasks vital to first graders and how different task contexts impact their writing performance. Therefore, the aim of this study is to explore the influence of handwriting prerequisites, including fine motor skills, visual perception, visual-motor integration, and working memory, on the performance of first-grade children in different writing tasks.
Method: The study recruited 30 children in the last year of kindergarten, assessing their handwriting prerequisites; the children's writing performance would be evaluated while in the first grade. Fine motor, visual perception, visual motor integration, and working memory performance were assessed as handwriting prerequisites. In addition, children were instructed to complete four copying tasks and Battery of Chinese Basic Literacy (BCBL) as their writing performance. Hierarchical regression analysis was adopted to analyze the predictive value. Results and conclusion: The study findings revealed that visual perception during the preschool period can effectively predict performance in the BCBL, with working memory also being a significant predictor and contributing to improved explanatory power when added to the model. Moreover, visual perception and visual motor integration can effectively predict the legibility of familiar copying in first graders. Among the four copying tasks, visual perception plays a crucial role in the accuracy of delayed copying, while the accuracy of copying unfamiliar characters and the still time of per stroke in the four copying tasks are strongly influenced by working memory. In the future, interventions or teaching strategies targeted at preschool children can be tailored to address the relevant predictive factors, which can effectively prevent potential handwriting difficulties. Additionally, attention could be paid to the impact of working memory on overall handwriting performance and stroke pauses during copying tasks. | en |
dc.description.provenance | Submitted by admin ntu (admin@lib.ntu.edu.tw) on 2023-09-22T16:12:08Z No. of bitstreams: 0 | en |
dc.description.provenance | Made available in DSpace on 2023-09-22T16:12:08Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 0 | en |
dc.description.tableofcontents | Table of Contents
致謝 i 中文摘要 II ABSTRACT III Table of Contents V List of Figures VII List of Tables VIII PREFACE 1 CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION 1 1.1 The importance of handwriting to school-age children 1 1.2.1 Formats of handwriting tasks 2 1.2.2 Evaluation of handwriting 4 1.2.2.1 Production analysis 4 1.2.2.2 Dynamic process analysis 6 1.3 Handwriting prerequisites 7 1.3.1 Fine motor (FM) 8 1.3.2 Visual perception (VP) 8 1.3.3 Visual motor integration (VMI) 10 1.3.4 Working memory (WM) 11 1.3.4.1 The definition of working memory and Baddeley’s model 11 1.3.4.2 Relationship between WM and handwriting performance 13 1.3.5 Handwriting prerequisites in longitudinal studies 15 1.4 Knowledge gap 16 1.5 Research purpose and hypothesis 16 CHAPTER 2. METHODS 18 2.1 Participants 18 2.2 Evaluation 18 2.2.1 Handwriting prerequisites 18 2.2.1.1 Fine Motor: A tracing task—Computerized Perceptual Motor Skills Assessment 18 2.2.1.2 Visual Perception 19 2.2.1.3 Visual motor integration 20 2.2.1.4 Working memory 20 2.2.2 Handwriting performance 22 2.2.2.1 Battery of Chinese Basic Literacy (BCBL) 23 2.2.2.2 The Tablet-based Evaluation of Chinese Handwriting (TECH) 23 2.4 Data analysis 28 CHAPTER 3. RESULTS 29 3.1 Demographic data 29 3.2 Predictors of different handwriting tasks 29 CHAPTER 4. DISCUSSION 32 4.1 The contribution of Perceptual Motor skills to different handwriting tasks (Model 1) 33 4.2 The role of working memory among different handwriting tasks (Model 2) 35 4.3 Limitations and future studies 39 CHEPTER 5. CONCLUSIONS 40 List of Figures Figure1. The working memory model revised from Baddeley’s model (2012). 41 Figure 2. Tracing task: fence shape of Computerized Perceptual Motor Skills Assessment 42 Figure 3. The schematic diagram of phonological working memory task 43 Figure 4. The schematic diagram of visual spatial working memory task 44 Figure 5. Four domains of character legibility: 45 | - |
dc.language.iso | en | - |
dc.title | 以幼兒園孩童工作記憶及知覺動作技巧作為其小一書寫表現之預測因子探討 | zh_TW |
dc.title | Working Memory and Perceptual Motor Skills in Kindergarten as Predictors of Handwriting Performance in First-Grade Children | en |
dc.type | Thesis | - |
dc.date.schoolyear | 111-2 | - |
dc.description.degree | 碩士 | - |
dc.contributor.oralexamcommittee | 王湉妮;吳建德;尤菀薈;盧璐 | zh_TW |
dc.contributor.oralexamcommittee | Tien-Ni Wang;Chien-Te Wu;Wan-Hui Yu;Lu Lu | en |
dc.subject.keyword | 工作記憶,視知覺動作技巧,書寫表現,小一,學齡前, | zh_TW |
dc.subject.keyword | working memory,visual perceptual motor skills,,handwriting performance,preschool,first grade, | en |
dc.relation.page | 70 | - |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.6342/NTU202304093 | - |
dc.rights.note | 同意授權(限校園內公開) | - |
dc.date.accepted | 2023-08-11 | - |
dc.contributor.author-college | 醫學院 | - |
dc.contributor.author-dept | 職能治療學系 | - |
dc.date.embargo-lift | 2028-08-10 | - |
顯示於系所單位: | 職能治療學系 |
文件中的檔案:
檔案 | 大小 | 格式 | |
---|---|---|---|
ntu-111-2.pdf 目前未授權公開取用 | 1.44 MB | Adobe PDF | 檢視/開啟 |
系統中的文件,除了特別指名其著作權條款之外,均受到著作權保護,並且保留所有的權利。