請用此 Handle URI 來引用此文件:
http://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/63147
完整後設資料紀錄
DC 欄位 | 值 | 語言 |
---|---|---|
dc.contributor.advisor | 陳淑惠(Sue-Huei Chen) | |
dc.contributor.author | Yu-Hsiang Chou | en |
dc.contributor.author | 周裕翔 | zh_TW |
dc.date.accessioned | 2021-06-16T16:25:03Z | - |
dc.date.available | 2014-02-01 | |
dc.date.copyright | 2013-02-01 | |
dc.date.issued | 2013 | |
dc.date.submitted | 2013-01-22 | |
dc.identifier.citation | 中文文獻:
周嘉娸(2008)。「憂鬱反芻對工作記憶容量的影響之初探」(未出版之碩士論文)。台北:國立臺灣大學心理學研究所。 游勝翔(2004)。「反芻型反應風格、自傳式記憶與憂鬱之關聯性探討」(未出版之碩士論文)。台北:國立臺灣大學心理學研究所。 游勝翔(2011)。「憂鬱反芻反應之促發與維持因素:探討憂鬱疾病狀態對個體憂鬱反芻調控歷程之影響」(未出版之博士論文)。台北:國立臺灣大學心理學研究所。 游勝翔、陳淑惠、張金堅(2008)。乳癌患者之反芻反應風格對其術後憂鬱與創傷後壓力症狀之預測。「中華心理學刊」,50,289-302。 盧孟良、車先蕙、張尚文、沈武典(2002)。中文版貝克憂鬱量表第二版之信度和效度。「臺灣精神醫學」,16,301-310。 英文文獻: Abela, J., Vanderbilt, E., & Rochon, A. (2004). A test of the integration of the response styles and social support theories of depression in third and seventh grade children. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 653-674. Ayduk, O., & Kross, E. (2010a). Analyzing negative experiences without ruminating: The role of self-distancing in enabling adaptive self-reflection. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 841-854. Ayduk, O., & Kross, E. (2010b). From a distance: Implications of spontaneous self-distancing for adaptive self-reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 809-829. doi: 10.1037/a0019205 Bagby, R. M., & Parker, J. D. A. (2001). Relation of rumination and distraction with neuroticism and extraversion in a sample of patients with major depression. Cognit Ther Res, 25, 91-102. Bar-Anan, Y., Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2006). The association between psychological distance and construal level: Evidence from an implicit association test. J Exp Psychol Gen, 135, 609-622. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.609 Bar-Anan, Y., Liberman, N., Trope, Y., & Algom, D. (2007). Automatic processing of psychological distance: Evidence from a Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 610-622. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.610 Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Ball, R., & Ranieri, W. (1996). Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67, 588-597. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13 Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., & Brown, G.K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory manual (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Beukeboom, C. J., & Semin, G. R. (2005). Mood and representations of behaviour: The how and why. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 1242-1251. Broderick, P. C., & Korteland, C. (2004). A prospective study of rumination and depression in early adolescence. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 9, 383-394. doi: 10.1177/1359104504043920 Burwell, R. A., & Shirk, S. R. (2007). Subtypes of rumination in adolescence: Associations between brooding, reflection, depressive symptoms, and coping. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 56-65. Calmes, C. A., & Roberts, J. E. (2007). Repetitive thought and emotional distress: Rumination and worry as prospective predictors of depressive and anxious symptomatology. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31, 343-356. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9026-9 Chan, S., Miranda, R., & Surrence, K. (2009). Subtypes of rumination in the relationship between negative life events and suicidal ideation. Archives of Suicide Research, 13, 123-135. doi: 10.1080/13811110902835015 Ciesla, J. A., & Roberts, J. E. (2007). Rumination, negative cognition, and their interactive effects on depressed mood. Emotion, 7, 555-565. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.555 Clark, D. M. (1983). On the induction of depressed mood in the laboratory: Evaluation and comparison of the Velten and musical procedures. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5, 27-49. Clark, D. M. & Teasdale, J. D. (1985). Constraints on the effects of mood on memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1595-1608. Conway, M., Csank, P. A., Holm, S. L., & Blake, C. K. (2000). On assessing individual differences in rumination on sadness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 75, 404-425. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA7503_04 Crane, C., Barnhofer, T., Visser, C., Nightingale, H., & Williams, J. M. (2007). The effects of analytical and experiential rumination on autobiographical memory specificity in individuals with a history of major depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 3077-3087. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.05.009 Debeer, E., Hermans, D., & Raes, F. (2009). Associations between components of rumination and autobiographical memory specificity as measured by a Minimal Instructions Autobiographical Memory Test. Memory, 17, 892-903. doi: 10.1080/09658210903376243 Donaldson, C., & Lam, D. (2004). Rumination, mood and social problem-solving in major depression. Psychological Medicine, 34, 1309-1318. doi:10.1017/s0033291704001904 Dozois, D. J. A., Dobson, K. S., & Ahnberg, J. L. (1998). A psychometric evaluation of the Beck Depression Inventory–II. Psychological Assessment, 10, 83-89. Ehring, T., Frank, S., & Ehlers, A. (2008). The role of rumination and reduced concreteness in the maintenance of posttraumatic stress disorder and depression following trauma. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 488-506. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9089-7 Forgas, J. P. (2007). When sad is better than happy: Negative affect can improve the quality and effectiveness of persuasive messages and social influence strategies. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 513-528. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.006 Giesbrecht, G. F., Muller, U., & Miller, M. (2010). Psychological distancing in the development of executive function and emotion regulation. In B. Sokol, U. Muller, A. Young, G. Iarocci, & J. Carpendale (Eds.), Self- and social-regulation: Exploring the relations between social interaction, social cognition, and the development of executive functions. Toronto, OX: Oxford University Press. Gird, S., & Zettle, R. D. (2009). Differential response to a dysphoric mood induction procedure as a function of level of experiential avoidance. The Psychological Record, 59, 537-550. Goldwin, M., & Behar, E. (2011). Concreteness of idiographic periods of worry and depressive rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36, 840-846. doi:10.1007/s10608-011-9428-1 Gortner, E. M., Rude, S. S., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2006). Benefits of expressive writing in lowering rumination and depressive symptoms. Behavior Therapy, 37, 292-303. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.01.004 Huffziger, S., & Kuehner, C. (2009). Rumination, distraction, and mindful self-focus in depressed patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 224-230. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.12.005 Hunt. (1998). The only way out is through: Emotional processing and recovery after a depressing life event. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 361-384. Joormann, J., Dkane, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination? Diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases. Behavior Therapy, 37, 269-280. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.01.002 Joormann, J., & Siemer, M. (2004). Memory accessibility, mood regulation, and dysphoria: Difficulties in repairing sad mood with happy memories? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 179-188. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.2.179 Katz, E. J., & Bertelson, A. D. (1993). The effects of gender and response style on depressed mood. Sex Roles, 29, 509-514. Keltenhauser, P. (1987). Zur experimentellen induktion von stimmungen. Ein vergleich der Velten-Technik mit einer kombination aus Velten-Technik und musikmethode (Unpublished master’s thesis). Universitat Gottingen, Germany. Kross, E. (2009). When the self becomes other: Toward an integrative understanding of the processes distinguishing adaptive self-reflection from rumination. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1167, 35-40. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04545.x Kross, E., & Ayduk, O. (2008). Facilitating adaptive emotional analysis: Distinguishing distanced-analysis of depressive experiences from immersed-analysis and distraction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 924-938. doi: 10.1177/0146167208315938 Kross, E., & Ayduk, O. (2011). Making meaning out of negative experiences by self-distancing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 187-191. doi: 10.1177/0963721411408883 Kross, E., Ayduk, O., & Mischel, W. (2005). When asking ‘‘why’’ does not hurt: Distinguishing rumination from reflective processing of negative emotions. Psychological Science, 16, 709-715. Kross, E., Gard, D., Deldin, P., Clifton, J., & Ayduk, O. (2012). 'Asking why' from a distance: Its cognitive and emotional consequences for people with major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 559-569. doi: 10.1037/a0028808 Kuehner, C., Holzhauer, S., & Huffziger, S. (2007). Decreased cortisol response to awakening is associated with cognitive vulnerability to depression in a nonclinical sample of young adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32, 199-209. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.12.007 Kuyken, W., Watkins, E., Holden, E., & Cook, W. (2006). Rumination in adolescents at risk for depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 96, 39-47. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.05.017 Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8. University of Florida, Gainesville, FL. Lavender, A., & Watkins, E. (2004). Rumination and future thinking in depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 129-142. Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, D. V., Weiller, E., Amorim, P., Bonora, I., Sheehan, K. H., Dunbar, G. C. (1997). The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI). A short diagnostic structured interview: Reliability and validity according to the CIDI. European Psychiatry, 12, 224-231. Lehmicke, N., & Robert, A.H. (1995). Relationship of response-set differences on Beck Depression Inventory scores of undergraduate students. Psychological Reports, 76, 15-21. Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 5-18. Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2008). The psychology of transcending the here and now. Science, 322, 1201-1205. doi: 10.1126/science.1161958 Liberman, N., Trope, Y., & Stephan, E. (2007). Psychological distance. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (Vol. 2, pp. 353–383). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Lyubomirsky, S., Caldwell, N. D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Effects of ruminative and distracting responses to depressed mood on retrieval of autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 166-177. Lyubomirsky, S., Kasri, F., & Zehm, K. (2003). Dysphoric rumination impairs concentration on academic tasks. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 309-330. Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Self-perpetuating properties of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 339-349. Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176-190. Lyubomirsky, S., Tucker, K. L., Caldwell, N. D., & Berg, K. (1999). Why ruminators are poor problem solvers: Clues from the phenomenology of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1041-1060. Marroquin, B. M., Fontes, M., Scilletta, A., & Miranda, R. (2010). Ruminative subtypes and coping responses: Active and passive pathways to depressive symptoms. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 1446-1455. doi: 10.1080/02699930903510212 McLaughlin, K. A., Borkovec, T. D., & Sibrava, N. J. (2007). The effects of worry and rumination on affect states and cognitive activity. Behavior Therapy, 38, 23-38. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.03.003 Mecklenbrauker, S., & Hager, W. (1984). Effects of mood on memory: Experimental tests of a mood-state-dependent retrieval hypothesis and of a mood-congruity hypothesis. Psychological Research, 46, 355-376. Miranda, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2007). Brooding and reflection: Rumination predicts suicidal ideation at 1-year follow-up in a community sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 3088-3095. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.07.015 Moberly, N. J., & Watkins, E. R. (2006). Processing mode influences the relationship between trait rumination and emotional vulnerability. Behavior Therapy, 37, 281-291. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.02.003 Moberly, N. J., & Watkins, E. R. (2008). Ruminative self-focus and negative affect: An experience sampling study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 314-323. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.314 Morrison, R., & O'Connor, R. C. (2008). The role of rumination, attentional biases and stress in psychological distress. British Journal of Psychology, 99, 191-209. doi: 10.1348/000712607X216080 Morrow, J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Effects of responses to depression on the remediation of depressive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 519-527. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 259-282. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569-582. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 504-511. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., McBride, A., & Larson, J. (1997). Rumination and psychological distress among bereaved partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 855-862. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta Earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115-121. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 561-570. doi: 10.1080/02699939308409206 Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 20-28. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L. E., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 92-104. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B.E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400-424. O'Connor, R. C., & Noyce, R. (2008). Personality and cognitive processes: Self-criticism and different types of rumination as predictors of suicidal ideation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 392-401. Raes, F., Hermans, D., Williams, J. M., Demyttenaere, K., Sabbe, B., Pieters, G., & Eelen, P. (2005). Reduced specificity of autobiographical memory: A mediator between rumination and ineffective social problem-solving in major depression? Journal of Affective Disorders, 87, 331-335. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.05.004 Rimes, K. A., & Watkins, E. (2005). The effects of self-focused rumination on global negative self-judgements in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1673-1681. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.12.002 Roberts, J. E., Gilboa, E., & Gotlib, I. H. (1998). Ruminative response style and vulnerability to episodes of dysphoria: Gender, neuroticism, and episode duration. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 401-423. Rude, S. S., Maestas, L. K., & Neff, K. (2007). Paying attention to distress: What's wrong with rumination? Cognition & Emotion, 21, 843-864. doi: 10.1080/02699930601056732 Schoofs, H., Hermans, D., & Raes, F. (2010). Brooding and reflection as subtypes of rumination: Evidence from confirmatory factor analysis in nonclinical samples using the dutch ruminative response scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 609-617. doi: 10.1007/s10862-010-9182-9 Schwartz, J., & Koenig, L. (1996). Response styles and negative affect among adolescents. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 20, 13-36. Segerstrom, S. C., Tsao, J. C. I., Alden, L. E., & Craske, M. G. (2000). Worry and rumination: Repetitive thought as a concomitant and predictor of negative mood. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 671-688. Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., Keskiner, A., Dunbar, G. C. (1997). The validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) according to the SCID-P and its reliability. European Psychiatry, 12, 232-241. Singer, A. R., & Dobson, K. S. (2007). An experimental investigation of the cognitive vulnerability to depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 563-575. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.007 Stober, J., & Borkovec, T. D. (2002). Reduced concreteness of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: Findings from a therapy study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 89-96. Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., & Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: Empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 275-287. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.2.275 Trask, P. C., & Sigmon, S. T. (1999). Ruminating and distracting: The effects of sequential tasks on depressed mood. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 23, 231-246. Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247-259. Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110, 403-421. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.110.3.403 Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117, 440-463. doi: 10.1037/a0018963 Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 3-15. Vallacher, R. R., Wegner, D. M., & Frederick, J. (1987). The presentation of self through action identification. Social Cognition, 5, 301-322. Van Boven, L., Kane, J., McGraw, A. P., & Dale, J. (2010). Feeling close: Emotional intensity reduces perceived psychological distance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 872-885. doi: 10.1037/a0019262 Vickers, K. S., & Vogeltanz-Holm, N. D. (2003). The effects of rumination and distraction tasks on psychophysiological responses and mood in dysphoric and nondysphoric individuals. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 331-348. Ward, A., Lyubomirsky, S., Sousa, L., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Can't quite commit: Rumination and uncertainty. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 96-107. doi: 10.1177/0146167202238375 Watkins, E. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive ruminative self-focus during emotional processing. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1037-1052. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.01.009 Watkins, E. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163-206. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163 Watkins, E. (2011). Dysregulation in level of goal and action identification across psychological disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 260-278. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.05.004 Watkins, E., Moberly, N. J., & Moulds, M. L. (2008). Processing mode causally influences emotional reactivity: Distinct effects of abstract versus concrete construal on emotional response. Emotion, 8, 364-378. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.364 Watkins, E., Moberly, N. J., & Moulds, M. L. (2011). When the ends outweigh the means: Mood and level of identification in depression. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 1214-1227. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.532389 Watkins, E., & Moulds, M. L. (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self-focus: Impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion, 5, 319-328. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.319 Watkins, E., & Moulds, M. L. (2007). Reduced concreteness of rumination in depression: A pilot study. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1386-1395. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.007 Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Rumination and overgeneral memory in depression: Effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 353-357. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.2.353 Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82, 1-8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2003.10.006 Watkins, E., Teasdale, J. D., & Williams, J. M. (2000). Decentring and distraction reduce overgeneral autobiographical memory in depression. Psychological Medicine, 30, 911-920. Whitmer, A., & Gotlib, I. H. (2011). Brooding and reflection reconsidered: A factor analytic examination of rumination in currently depressed, formerly depressed, and never depressed individuals. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 99-107. doi: 10.1007/s10608-011-9361-3 Wisco, B. E., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2009). The interaction of mood and rumination in depression: Effects on mood maintenance and mood-congruent autobiographical memory. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 27, 144-159. doi: 10.1007/s10942-009-0096-y Wisco, B. E., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Effect of visual perspective on memory and interpretation in dysphoria. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 406-412. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2011.03.012 | |
dc.identifier.uri | http://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/63147 | - |
dc.description.abstract | 前言:過去文獻指出,反芻狀態與反芻風格可區分成具適應性及不具適應性之面向,而個體在反芻前具較高之憂鬱情緒、反芻歷程中採用較遠的心理距離和較不具體之概念層次,以及擁有較強的苦惱自責反芻次風格傾向,皆與反芻不具適應性之面向有關。然而,過去研究未直接比較憂鬱反芻與非憂鬱反芻在認知歷程中之差異,且多以實驗操弄進行研究,未瞭解個體於反芻狀態中心理距離及概念層次之自然現象與憂鬱情緒間的關係,亦較少同時考量反芻狀態與風格之影響。目的:本研究企圖以實驗誘發不同之反芻狀態(即,憂鬱反芻與非憂鬱反芻),並納入反芻風格之考量,以兩個子研究來探究兩者對反芻歷程中心理距離及概念層次的影響。方法:本研究以純音樂誘發個體之負向或非負向(中性�正向)情緒,之後以反芻書寫的方式使其分別對應進入憂鬱反芻或非憂鬱反芻之狀態中,反芻後則使用0至100之視覺等化量尺進行情緒狀態及心理距離之自評。在實驗結束後,本研究分析參與者反芻書寫內容之具體程度以為概念層次之指標。研究一採用102名大學生為樣本,以「個體之負向人際經驗」為反芻內容;研究二則採用70名大學生為樣本,以實驗中創造之「相同負向經驗」為反芻內容,以控制個人經驗之客觀因素所造成的影響。兩個子研究的參與者除了實驗歷程的評估外,也施以中文版反芻反應風格量表短版(C_RSQ_r10)測量個體之反芻風格�次風格,以及中文版貝克憂鬱量表第二版測量個體近兩週內之憂鬱程度。結果:(1)反芻狀態方面,心理距離部分,研究一以「個體負向人際經驗」為內容的憂鬱反芻狀態中之時間、空間心理距離較非憂鬱反芻遠;研究二以「相同負向經驗」為內容的憂鬱反芻中之時間、空間、自我等距離則較非憂鬱反芻組近。概念層次部分,參與者反芻書寫內容之具體程度在兩個子研究中皆無顯著組間差異。(2)反芻風格方面,心理距離部分,研究一參與者之反芻風格和次風格皆與心理距離無關;研究二參與者之深思反省次風格與時間、空間心理距離呈正相關之趨勢,苦惱自責次風格則與空間、自我心理距離呈負相關。概念層次部分,僅研究一之高憂鬱程度者(BDI-II > 13)的反芻風格總分及深思反省次風格,與反芻內容之具體程度皆呈顯著負相關。討論:以研究樣本特性、反芻前音樂情緒誘發、及反芻持續時間之實驗設計,並引用心理距離之概念層次理論(Liberman & Trope, 1998)及識別層次失調假設(Watkins, 2011)等角度探究反芻狀態與風格對結果變項的影響,最後,本論文也討論臨床之應用與未來可行之研究方向。 | zh_TW |
dc.description.abstract | Introduction: Literature review shows that ruminative states and styles can be divided in the adaptive or maladaptive forms, i.e., reflection and brooding, respectively. Several conditions are found to be related to the maladaptive states (here referred to depression), including ruminating with higher depression, farther psychological distance, less concreteness of the construal levels, and a tendency toward brooding ruminative style. Nevertheless, it remains unclear that: (a) whether non-depressive ruminative state is more adaptive than depressive ruminative state, (b) what would be the natural phenomena of psychological distance and construal levels during rumination, and (c) whether different combinations of ruminative states and styles have different outcomes. Purpose: The present thesis, with two studies using private negative interpersonal experience and common negative experience, respectively, aimed to empirically investigate the effects of depressive/non-depressive ruminative states and ruminative styles on the psychological distance and construal levels during rumination. Method: Recruited undergraduate students were randomly assigned to “negative mood induction” (depressive ruminative group[DR]) or “non-negative mood induction ” (non-depressive ruminative group[nDR]) procedure by listening to the music without lyrics. Participants (n = 102) in Study 1 were invited to write and ruminate on the topic of “private negative interpersonal experience”. In Study 2, participants (n = 70) ruminate on the topic of “the common negative experience”, which was created in the experiment to control the confounding effect of occurrence time, place and content of personal event. After rumination task, participants rated their sadness, happiness and psychological distance on visual analogue scale (0 to 100), then completed the Chinese Version of Response Styles Questionnaire_Rumination_10 items[C_RSQ_r10] for measuring the tendency of ruminative styles and Beck Depression Inventory-II for depression measure. At the end, debriefing and thanks were given. In terms of index for construal levels, two raters scored the concreteness of the rumination writing contents, which was examined to meet satisfactory agreement. Results: (1) Considering the ruminative states, in Study 1, the temporal and special psychological distance in DR were greater than in nDR. However, in Study 2, the findings were different, and specifically, temporal, special and self distance were closer in DR than in nDR. The concreteness of the rumination content was not different between DR and nDR in both two studies. (2) Concerning the correlation between ruminative styles and psychological distance, there were no significant results in Study 1. However, in Study 2, participants’ reflective ruminative style was found to correlate with farther temporal and special psychological distance, and brooding ruminative style was significantly correlated with the closer special and self psychological distance. Of the correlation between ruminative style and construal level, only individuals with higher depression (i.e., BDI-II > 13) in Study 1 displayed negative corrections of C_RSQ_r10 total score and reflective ruminative style with concreteness of rumination contents. Discussion and Conclusions: Several possible limitations such as sample characteristics, musical mood induction procedure, and time of rumination were considered. The discussion on the findings was addressed. Finally, possible clinical applications and future research directions were further provided. | en |
dc.description.provenance | Made available in DSpace on 2021-06-16T16:25:03Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1 ntu-102-R98227203-1.pdf: 849742 bytes, checksum: 5eb72b9149161d4ac5f04befe62a79b5 (MD5) Previous issue date: 2013 | en |
dc.description.tableofcontents | 第一章 緒論..............................................................................................1
第一節 憂鬱反芻..................................................................................................2 第二節 心理距離與憂鬱心情..............................................................................7 第三節 概念層次與憂鬱心情............................................................................10 第四節 研究目的與假設....................................................................................14 第二章 研究方法與結果.........................................................................19 第一節 研究一:憂鬱反芻與風格對心理距離及概念層次的影響 -以「個人之負向人際經驗」為反芻內容........................19 第二節 研究二:憂鬱反芻與風格對心理距離及概念層次的影響 -以「相同負向經驗」為反芻內容....................................35 第三章 綜合討論..............................................................................51 第一節 反芻狀態、反芻風格與正負向情緒改變............................................51 第二節 反芻狀態、反芻風格與心理距離........................................................55 第三節 反芻狀態、反芻風格與概念層次........................................................58 第四章 研究貢獻、限制與未來研究方向.............................................63 第一節 研究貢獻................................................................................................63 第二節 研究限制................................................................................................64 第三節 未來研究方向........................................................................................65 參考文獻.......................................................................................................66 附錄................................................................................................................75 附錄一..................................................................................................................75 附錄二..................................................................................................................76 附錄三..................................................................................................................77 | |
dc.language.iso | zh-TW | |
dc.title | 憂鬱反芻對心理距離與概念層次的影響 | zh_TW |
dc.title | The Effects of Depressive Rumination on Psychological Distance and Construal Levels in Non-Clinical Samples | en |
dc.type | Thesis | |
dc.date.schoolyear | 101-1 | |
dc.description.degree | 碩士 | |
dc.contributor.oralexamcommittee | 洪福建(Fu-Chien Hung),李錦虹(Gin-Hong Lee) | |
dc.subject.keyword | 心理距離,概念層次,憂鬱反芻,反芻風格, | zh_TW |
dc.subject.keyword | Psychological Distance,Construal Level,Depressive Rumination,Ruminative Style, | en |
dc.relation.page | 77 | |
dc.rights.note | 有償授權 | |
dc.date.accepted | 2013-01-22 | |
dc.contributor.author-college | 理學院 | zh_TW |
dc.contributor.author-dept | 心理學研究所 | zh_TW |
顯示於系所單位: | 心理學系 |
文件中的檔案:
檔案 | 大小 | 格式 | |
---|---|---|---|
ntu-102-1.pdf 目前未授權公開取用 | 829.83 kB | Adobe PDF |
系統中的文件,除了特別指名其著作權條款之外,均受到著作權保護,並且保留所有的權利。