請用此 Handle URI 來引用此文件:
http://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/1317
完整後設資料紀錄
DC 欄位 | 值 | 語言 |
---|---|---|
dc.contributor.advisor | 薛漪平 | |
dc.contributor.author | Wan-Hui Yu | en |
dc.contributor.author | 尤菀薈 | zh_TW |
dc.date.accessioned | 2021-05-12T09:36:16Z | - |
dc.date.available | 2021-02-22 | |
dc.date.available | 2021-05-12T09:36:16Z | - |
dc.date.copyright | 2018-02-22 | |
dc.date.issued | 2018 | |
dc.date.submitted | 2018-01-11 | |
dc.identifier.citation | 參考文獻
心理測驗 (王文中、陳承德譯) (民98)。台北市:雙葉。 林鋐宇、張文典、洪福源(民100)。注意力的神經生理機制。身心障礙研究,2,123-134。 黃敏偉、陳萱佳 (民102)。創造雙贏“精神分裂症'更名 需要大家支持。民國104年4月28日,取自:http://www.sop.org.tw/Official/official_08.asp 鄭昭明 (民90)。認知心理學:理論與實踐 (再修訂三版)。台北市:桂冠圖書。 鄭麗玉 (民95)。認知心理學:理論與應用 (第三版)。台北市:五南圖書出版股份有限公司。 Assef, E. C. d. S., Capovilla, A. G. S., & Capovilla, F. C. (2007). Computerized Stroop test to assess selective attention in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10, 33-40. doi:10.1017/S1138741600006296. Barch, D. M., & Carter, C. S. (1998). Selective attention in schizophrenia: relationship to verbal working memory. Schizophrenia Research, 33, 53-61. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00064-4. Barnett, J. H., Robbins, T. W., Leeson, V. C., Sahakian, B. J., Joyce, E. M., & Blackwell, A. D. (2010). Assessing cognitive function in clinical trials of schizophrenia. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 1161-1177. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.012. Bartels, C., Wegrzyn, M., Wiedl, A., Ackermann, V., & Ehrenreich, H. (2010). Practice effects in healthy adults: a longitudinal study on frequent repetitive cognitive testing. BMC Neuroscience, 11, 118. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-11-118. Baudouin, J. Y., Martin, F., Tiberghien, G., Verlut, I., & Franck, N. (2002). Selective attention to facial emotion and identity in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia, 40, 503-511. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00114-2. Beglinger, L. J., Gaydos, B., Tangphao-Daniels, O., Duff, K., Kareken, D. A., Crawford, J., . . . Siemers, E. R. (2005). Practice effects and the use of alternate forms in serial neuropsychological testing. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 517-529. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2004.12.003. Berberian, A. A., Trevisan, B. T., Moriyama, T. S., Montiel, J. M., Oliveira, J. A. C., & Seabra, A. G. (2009). Working memory assessment in schizophrenia and its correlation with executive functions ability. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 31, 219-226. doi:10.1590/S1516-44462009000300007. Brebion, G., Smith, M. J., Gorman, J. M., & Amador, X. (1996). Reality monitoring failure in schizophrenia: the role of selective attention. Schizophrenia Research, 22, 173-180. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(96)00054-0. Brissos, S., Molodynski, A., Dias, V. V., & Figueira, M. L. (2011). The importance of measuring psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia. Annals of General Psychiatry, 10, 18. doi:10.1186/1744-859X-10-18. Broadbent, D. E. (1954). The role of auditory localization in attention and memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 51-55. doi:10.1037/h0054182. Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and Communication. New York: Oxford University Press. Burns, T., & Patrick, D. (2007). Social functioning as an outcome measure in schizophrenia studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 116, 403-418. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01108.x. Bushnell, C. D., Johnston, D. C., & Goldstein, L. B. (2001). Retrospective assessment of initial stroke severity: comparison of the NIH Stroke Scale and the Canadian Neurological Scale. Stroke, 32, 656-660. doi:10.1161/01.STR.32.3.656. Canini, M., Battista, P., Della Rosa, P. A., Catricala, E., Salvatore, C., Gilardi, M. C., & Castiglioni, I. (2014). Computerized neuropsychological assessment in aging: testing efficacy and clinical ecology of different interfaces. Computational and Mathematical Methods in Medicine, 2014, 804723. doi:10.1155/2014/804723. Chaytor, N., & Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2003). The ecological validity of neuropsychological tests: a review of the literature on everyday cognitive skills. Neuropsychology Review, 13, 181-197. doi:10.1023/B:NERV.0000009483.91468.fb. Chen, H. C., Koh, C. L., Hsieh, C. L., & Hsueh, I. P. (2013). Test of Everyday Attention in patients with chronic stroke: test-retest reliability and practice effects. Brain Injury, 27, 1148-1154. doi: 10.3109/02699052.2013.775483. Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975-979. doi:10.1121/1.1907229. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Collie, A., Maruff, P., Darby, D. G., & McStephen, M. (2003). The effects of practice on the cognitive test performance of neurologically normal individuals assessed at brief test-retest intervals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9, 419-428. doi:10.1017/s1355617703930074. Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281-302. doi:10.1037/h0040957. Deutsch, J. A., & Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70, 51-60. doi:10.1037/h0042712. Dikmen, S. S., Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Temkin, N. R. (1999). Test-retest reliability and practice effects of expanded Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 346-356. doi:10.1017/S1355617799544056. DiStefano, C., & Hess, B. (2005). Using confirmatory factor analysis for construct validation: an empirical review. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 23, 225-241. doi:10.1177/073428290502300303. Egeland, J., Rund, B. R., Sundet, K., Landro, N. I., Asbjornsen, A., Lund, A., . . . Hugdahl, K. (2003). Attention profile in schizophrenia compared with depression: differential effects of processing speed, selective attention and vigilance. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 108, 276-284. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.00146.x. Engelhardt, N. (1994). Selective visual attention to novelty in elderly with senile dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Case Western Reserve University. Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143-149. doi:10.3758/BF03203267. Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340-347. doi:10.1162/089892902317361886. Fioravanti, M., Bianchi, V., & Cinti, M. E. (2012). Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: an updated metanalysis of the scientific evidence. BMC Psychiatry, 12, 64. doi:10.1186/1471-244x-12-64 Fioravanti, M., Carlone, O., Vitale, B., Cinti, M. E., & Clare, L. (2005). A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in adults with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychology Review, 15, 73-95. doi:10.1007/s11065-005-6254-9 Fischer, E. P., Shumway, M., & Owen, R. R. (2002). Priorities of consumers, providers, and family members in the treatment of schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services, 53, 724-729. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.53.6.724. Flansbjer, U. B., Holmback, A. M., Downham, D., & Lexell, J. (2005). What change in isokinetic knee muscle strength can be detected in men and women with hemiparesis after stroke? Clinical Rehabilitation, 19, 514-522. doi:10.1191/0269215505cr854oa. Forster, S., Robertson, D. J., Jennings, A., Asherson, P., & Lavie, N. (2014). Plugging the attention deficit: perceptual load counters increased distraction in ADHD. Neuropsychology, 28, 91-97. doi:10.1037/neu0000020. Frost, M. H., Reeve, B. B., Liepa, A. M., Stauffer, J. W., & Hays, R. D. (2007). What is sufficient evidence for the reliability and validity of patient-reported outcome measures? Value Health, 10 Suppl 2, S94-s105. doi:10.1111/j.1524-4733.2007.00272.x. Fusar-Poli, P., & Politi, P. (2008). Paul Eugen Bleuler and the birth of schizophrenia (1908). American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1407. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050714. Galaverna, F. S., Morra, C. A., & Bueno, A. M. (2012). Attention in patients with chronic schizophrenia: Deficit in inhibitory control and positive symptoms. European Journal of Psychiatry, 26, 185-195. doi:10.4321/S0213-61632012000300005. Goldberg, T. E., Goldman, R. S., Burdick, K. E., Malhotra, A. K., Lencz, T., Patel, R. C., . . . Robinson, D. G. (2007). Cognitive improvement after treatment with second-generation antipsychotic medications in first-episode schizophrenia: is it a practice effect? Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 1115-1122. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.64.10.1115. Goldberg, T. E., Harvey, P. D., Wesnes, K. A., Snyder, P. J., & Schneider, L. S. (2015). Practice effects due to serial cognitive assessment: implications for preclinical Alzheimer's disease randomized controlled trials. Alzheimer's & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, 1, 103-111. doi:10.1016/j.dadm.2014.11.003. Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test: a manual for clinical and experimental uses. Chicago, IL: Stoelting Co. Gooding, D. C., Braun, J. G., & Studer, J. A. (2006). Attentional network task performance in patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders: evidence of a specific deficit. Schizophrenia Research, 88, 169-178. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.07.009. Gualtieri, C. T., & Johnson, L. G. (2006). Reliability and validity of a computerized neurocognitive test battery, CNS Vital Signs. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 623-643. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2006.05.007. Guy, W. (1976). ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology, Revised. US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare Publication (ADM). Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health. Haley, S. M., & Fragala-Pinkham, M. A. (2006). Interpreting change scores of tests and measures used in physical therapy. Physical Therapy, 86, 735-743. doi:10.1093/ptj/86.5.735. Hunter, J. P., Marshall, R. N., & McNair, P. (2004). Reliability of biomechanical variables of sprint running. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 36, 850-861. doi:10.1249/01.MSS.0000126467.58091.38. Jablensky, A. (2000). Epidemiology of schizophrenia: the global burden of disease and disability. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 250, 274-285. doi:10.1007/s004060070002. James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology (Vol. 1). New York, NY: Holt. Jette, A. M., Tao, W., Norweg, A., & Haley, S. (2007). Interpreting rehabilitation outcome measurements. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 39, 585-590. doi:10.2340/16501977-0119 Johnston, W. A., & Heinz, S. P. (1978). Flexibility and capacity demands of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 420. doi:10.1037//0096-3445.107.4.420. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Pentice-hall Inc. Kucharska-Pietura, K., Tylec, A., Czernikiewicz, A., & Mortimer, A. (2012). Attentional and emotional functioning in schizophrenia patients treated with conventional and atypical antipsychotic drugs. Medical Science Monitor, 18, Cr44-49. doi:10.12659/MSM.882202. Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 451-468. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.21.3.451. Lavie, N. (2000). Selective attention and cognitive control: Dissociating attentional functions through different types of load. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 175-194). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT press. Lavie, N. (2001). The role of capacity limits in selective attention: behavioural evidence and implications for neural activity. In J. Braun & C. Koch (Eds.), Visual Attention and Cortical Circuits (pp. 49-68). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT press. Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 75-82. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004. Lavie, N., Beck, D. M., & Konstantinou, N. (2014). Blinded by the load: attention, awareness and the role of perceptual load. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 369, 20130205. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0205. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0205. Lavie, N., & Cox, S. (1997). On the efficiency of visual selective attention: Efficient visual search leads to inefficient distractor rejection. Psychological Science, 8, 395-396. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00432.x. Lavie, N., & Tsal, Y. (1994). Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 183-197. doi:10.3758/BF03213897. Lenior, M. E., Dingemans, P. M., Linszen, D. H., de Haan, L., & Schene, A. H. (2001). Social functioning and the course of early-onset schizophrenia: five-year follow-up of a psychosocial intervention. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 53-58. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.1.53. Leucht, S., & Engel, R. R. (2006). The relative sensitivity of the Clinical Global Impressions Scale and the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale in antipsychotic drug trials. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31, 406-412. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300873. Lexell, J. E., & Downham, D. Y. (2005). How to assess the reliability of measurements in rehabilitation. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 84, 719-723. doi:10.1097/01.phm.0000176452.17771.20. Lezak, M., Howieson, D., & Loring, D. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York:: Oxford university press. Lowe, C., & Rabbitt, P. (1998). Test/re-test reliability of the CANTAB and ISPOCD neuropsychological batteries: theoretical and practical issues. Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery. International Study of Post-Operative Cognitive Dysfunction. Neuropsychologia, 36, 915-923. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00036-0. Luck, S. J., & Vecera, S. P. (2002). Attention. In S. Yantis & H. Pashler (Eds.), Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology (Vol. 1): Sensation and Perception (3rd ed., pp. 235-286). New York: Wiley. Mayer, R. E. (1981). The promise of cognitive psychology. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Company. McCaffrey, R. J., Duff, K., & Westervelt, H. J. (2000). Practitioner's guide to evaluating change with neuropsychological assessment instruments. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. McGhie, A., & Chapman, J. (1961). Disorders of attention and perception in early schizophrenia. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 34, 103-116. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8341.1961.tb00936.x. McGrath, J., Saha, S., Welham, J., El Saadi, O., MacCauley, C., & Chant, D. (2004). A systematic review of the incidence of schizophrenia: the distribution of rates and the influence of sex, urbanicity, migrant status and methodology. BMC Medicine, 2, 13. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-2-13. Mirsky, A. F. (1987). Behavioral and psychophysiological markers of disordered attention. Environmental Health Perspectives, 74, 191-199. doi:10.1289/ehp.8774191. Mirsky, A. F. (1996). Disorders of attention: A neuropsychological perspective. In G. R. Lyon & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 71-95). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. Mirsky, A. F., Anthony, B. J., Duncan, C. C., Ahearn, M. B., & Kellam, S. G. (1991). Analysis of the elements of attention: a neuropsychological approach. Neuropsychology Review, 2, 109-145. doi:10.1007/BF01109051. Mirsky, A. F., & Duncan, C. C. (2001). A nosology of disorders of attention. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 931, 17-32. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05771.x. Mirsky, A. F., Yardley, S. L., Jones, B. P., Walsh, D., & Kendler, K. S. (1995). Analysis of the Attention-Deficit in Schizophrenia: a study of patients and their relatives in Ireland. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 29, 23-42. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(94)00041-O. Moering, R. G., Schinka, J. A., Mortimer, J. A., & Graves, A. B. (2004). Normative data for elderly African Americans for the Stroop color and word test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 61-71. doi:10.1016/S0887-6177(02)00219-6. Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 56-60. doi:10.1080/17470215908416289. Morosini, P. L., Magliano, L., Brambilla, L., Ugolini, S., & Pioli, R. (2000). Development, reliability and acceptability of a new version of the DSM-IV Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale (SOFAS) to assess routine social functioning. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 101, 323-329. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2000.tb10933.x. Morris, R., Griffiths, O., Le Pelley, M. E., & Weickert, T. W. (2013). Attention to irrelevant cues is related to positive symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39, 575-582. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr192. Morrow, S. A. (2013). Normative data for the Stroop Color Word Test for a North American population. The Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 40, 842-847. doi:10.1017/S0317167100015997. Murphy, G., Groeger, J. A., & Greene, C. M. (2016). Twenty years of load theory-Where are we now, and where should we go next? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 1316-1340. doi:10.3758/s13423-015-0982-5. Myles-Worsley, M., Coon, H., & Byerley, W. (1998). The sensitivity of the Spontaneous Selective Attention Test (SSAT): a study of schizophrenic inpatients and outpatients versus normal controls. Schizophrenia Research, 31, 131-139. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00012-7. Myles-Worsley, M., Johnston, W. A., & Wender, P. H. (1991). Spontaneous selective attention in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 39, 167-179. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(91)90085-4. Nafees, B., van Hanswijck de Jonge, P., Stull, D., Pascoe, K., Price, M., Clarke, A., & Turkington, D. (2012). Reliability and validity of the Personal and Social Performance scale in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 140, 71-76. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.06.013. Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bedirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., . . . Chertkow, H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53, 695-699. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. Picchioni, M. M., & Murray, R. M. (2007). Schizophrenia. BMJ, 335, 91-95. doi:10.1136/bmj.39227.616447.BE. Portney, L. G., & Watkins, M. P. (2009). Foundations of clinical research: applications to practice (Third Edition ed.). Upper Saddle River: Pearson Prentice Hall. Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3-25. doi:10.1080/00335558008248231. Posner, M. I. (2004). Cognitive neuroscience of attention. New York: The Guilford Press. Posner, M. I., & Boies, S. J. (1971). Components of attention. Psychological Review, 78, 391. doi:10.1037/h0031333. Posner, M. I., & Fernandez-Duque, D. (1999). Attention in the human brain. In R. A. Wilson & F. C. Keil (Eds.), The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25-42. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325. Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1996). Images of Mind. NY: Scientific American Library. Rees, G., Frith, C. D., & Lavie, N. (1997). Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. Science, 278, 1616-1619. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5343.1616. Robertson, I. H., Ward, T., Ridgeway, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1994). The test of everyday attention: TEA. Bury St. Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test Company. Rorden, C., Guerrini, C., Swainson, R., Lazzeri, M., & Baylis, G. C. (2008). Event related potentials reveal that increasing perceptual load leads to increased responses for target stimuli and decreased responses for irrelevant stimuli. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2:4. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.004.2008 Rubio Gomez, J. L., Hernandez Bellido, L., Ruiz Veguilla, M., Ferrin Erdozain, M., Pereda Banos, A., & Gomez Milan, E. (2010). Attentional network task performance in schizophrenic patients. Psicothema, 22, 664-668. Ruff, & Allen. (1996). Ruff 2 & 7 Selective Attention Test professional manual. Odessa, FI: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. Ruff, Niemann, H., Allen, C. C., Farrow, C. E., & Wylie, T. (1992). The Ruff 2 and 7 Selective Attention Test - a Neuropsychological Application. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 1311-1319. doi:10.2466/Pms.75.8.1311-1319. Rund, B. R., & Borg, N. (1999). Cognitive deficits and cognitive training in schizophrenic patients: a review. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 100, 85-95. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb10829.x. Saha, S., Chant, D., Welham, J., & McGrath, J. (2005). A systematic review of the prevalence of schizophrenia. PLoS Medicine, 2, e141. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0020141. Schaub, A., Neubauer, N., Mueser, K. T., Engel, R., & Moller, H. J. (2013). Neuropsychological functioning in inpatients with major depression or schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatry, 13, 203. doi:10.1186/1471-244x-13-203 Seeman, P., & Kapur, S. (2000). Schizophrenia: more dopamine, more D2 receptors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 7673-7675. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.14.7673. Semkovska, M., Bedard, M. A., Godbout, L., Limoge, F., & Stip, E. (2004). Assessment of executive dysfunction during activities of daily living in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 69, 289-300. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.07.005. Seo, E. H., Lee, D. Y., Kim, S. G., Kim, K. W., Youn, J. C., Jhoo, J. H., & Woo, J. I. (2008). Normative study of the Stroop Color and Word Test in an educationally diverse elderly population. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 23, 1020-1027. doi:10.1002/gps.2027. Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 298, 199-209. doi:10.1098/rstb.1982.0082. Silverberg, N. B., Ryan, L. M., Carrillo, M. C., Sperling, R., Petersen, R. C., Posner, H. B., . . . Ferman, T. J. (2011). Assessment of cognition in early dementia. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7, e60-e76. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2011.05.001 Sohlberg, M. M., & Mateer, C. A. (1987). Effectiveness of an attention-training program. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, 117-130. doi:10.1080/01688638708405352. Sohlberg, M. M., & Mateer, C. A. (2001). Improving attention and managing attentional problems. Adapting rehabilitation techniques to adults with ADD. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 931, 359-375. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05790.x. Steinberg, B. A., Bieliauskas, L. A., Smith, G. E., & Ivnik, R. J. (2005). Mayo's older Americans normative studies: age-and IQ-adjusted norms for the trail-making test, the stroop test, and MAE controlled oral word association test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 19, 329-377. doi:10.1080/13854040590945210. Sternberg, R. J. , & Sternberg, K. (2016). Cognitive psychology (6 ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Stevens, C., & Bavelier, D. (2012). The role of selective attention on academic foundations: a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, S30-S48. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.11.001. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662. doi:10.1037/h0054651. Szoke, A., Meary, A., Ferchiou, A., Trandafir, A., Leboyer, M., & Schurhoff, F. (2009). Correlations between cognitive performances and psychotic or schizotypal dimensions. European Psychiatry, 24, 244-250. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.10.007 Thompson, P. M., Vidal, C., Giedd, J. N., Gochman, P., Blumenthal, J., Nicolson, R., . . . Rapoport, J. L. (2001). Mapping adolescent brain change reveals dynamic wave of accelerated gray matter loss in very early-onset schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 11650-11655. doi:10.1073/pnas.201243998. Treisman. (1960). Contextual cues in selective listening. Quarterly journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 242-248. doi:10.1080/17470216008416732. Treisman, & Davies. (1973). Divided attention to ear and eye. In S. Kornblum (Ed.), Attention and Performance IV (pp. 101-117). London: Academic Press. Treisman, & Geffen, G. (1967). Selective attention: perception or response? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19, 1-17. doi:10.1080/14640746708400062. Tung, L. C., Yu, W. H., Lin, G. H., Yu, T. Y., Wu, C. T., Tsai, C. Y., . . . Hsieh, C. L. (2016). Development of a Tablet-based symbol digit modalities test for reliably assessing information processing speed in patients with stroke. Disability and Rehabilitation, 38, 1952-1960. doi:10.3109/09638288.2015.1111438. Tupper, D., & Cicerone, K. (1990). Introduction to the neuropsychology of everyday life. In D. Tupper & K. Cicerone (Eds.), The neuropsychology of everyday life: Assessment and basic competencies (pp. 3-18). Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic. Tyson, P. J., Laws, K. R., Flowers, K. A., Mortimer, A. M., & Schulz, J. (2008). Attention and executive function in people with schizophrenia: Relationship with social skills and quality of life. International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 12, 112-119. doi:10.1080/13651500701687133. Van der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P., Van Breukelen, G. J., & Jolles, J. (2006). The Stroop Color-Word Test influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment, 13, 62-79. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283427. Van Horn, J., & McManus, I. (1992). Ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia. A meta-analysis of studies of the ventricle: brain ratio (VBR). The British Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 687-697. doi:10.1192/bjp.160.5.687. van Os, J., & Kapur, S. (2009). Schizophrenia. Lancet, 374, 635-645. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(09)60995-8. Vu, K. L. (2004). Historical overview of research on attention. In A. Johnson & R. Proctor (Eds.), Attention: Theory and practice (pp. 1-24). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Wang, K., Fan, J., Dong, Y., Wang, C. Q., Lee, T. M., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Selective impairment of attentional networks of orienting and executive control in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 78, 235-241. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.01.019. Waters, F. A., Badcock, J. C., & Maybery, M. T. (2006). Selective attention for negative information and depression in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 36, 455-464. doi:10.1017/s0033291705007026. Weir, J. P. (2005). Quantifying test-retest reliability using the intraclass correlation coefficient and the SEM. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 19, 231-240. doi:10.1519/15184.1. Weiss, K. M. (1996). A simple clinical assessment of attention in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 60, 147-154. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(96)02859-4. Wickens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Parasuraman & R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 63-101). New York: Academic Press. Wickens, C. D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3, 159-177. doi:10.1080/14639220210123806. Wu, C., Dagg, P., & Molgat, C. (2014). A pilot study to measure cognitive impairment in patients with severe schizophrenia with the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA). Schizophrenia Research, 158, 151-155. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2014.07.006 Zalonis, I., Christidi, F., Bonakis, A., Kararizou, E., Triantafyllou, N. I., Paraskevas, G., . . . Vasilopoulos, D. (2009). The stroop effect in Greek healthy population: normative data for the Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24, 81-88. doi:10.1093/arclin/acp011. Zehnder, A. E., Blasi, S., Berres, M., Spiegel, R., & Monsch, A. U. (2007). Lack of practice effects on neuropsychological tests as early cognitive markers of Alzheimer disease? American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease & Other Dementiasr, 22, 416-426. doi:10.1177/1533317507302448. Zeng, Z., Zeng, Z., Miao, C., Miao, C., Leung, C., Leung, C., . . . Shen, Z. (2017). Computerizing Trail Making Test for long-term cognitive self-assessment. International Journal of Crowd Science, 1, 83-99. doi:10.1108/IJCS-12-2016-0002. Zomeren, A. H., & Brouwer, W. H. (1994). Clinical neuropsychology of attention. New York: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.identifier.uri | http://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/handle/123456789/1317 | - |
dc.description.abstract | 背景與目的:選擇性注意力缺失常見於思覺失調症患者,影響其功能性表現,故評估患者之選擇性注意力有其必要性。知覺負載理論考量個體從事任務之知覺負荷量對注意力選擇機制之影響,並輔以神經生理證據,可將選擇性注意力之機制解釋地較為清楚。然而,目前缺乏以此理論及實驗典範為基礎而發展之選擇性注意力測驗,導致臨床人員無法深入評估思覺失調症患者之選擇性注意力問題。因此,本研究之目的有三:一、發展一套以知覺負載理論為理論基礎之iPad版選擇性注意力測驗 (iPad-based Selective Attention Test, iSAT),並建立其內容效度和確定其可行性;二、驗證iSAT於思覺失調症患者之心理計量特性,以提昇iSAT於思覺失調症患者之臨床與研究應用價值;三、驗證iSAT於20至29歲年輕成人之心理計量特性,確保iSAT於建立20至29歲常模前具備適當之心理計量特性。
研究方法:本研究包含三個子研究。研究一為iSAT之發展,並建立其內容效度和確定其可行性,分成三階段進行:(一) 依據知覺負載理論及相關實驗典範和專家意見建立iSAT初版內容及行動應用程式;(二) 募集20至29歲年輕成人和20歲以上思覺失調症患者測試iSAT初版之可行性,再依測試結果及專家意見修改成iSAT二版;(三) 募集20至29歲年輕成人和20歲以上思覺失調症患者測試iSAT二版之可行性,再依測試結果和專家意見確定iSAT正式版內容,進而建立iSAT之內容效度。研究二為iSAT於思覺失調症患者之心理計量特性驗證。研究者募集20歲以上思覺失調症患者參與二次iSAT評估(間隔二週),以檢驗iSAT應用於思覺失調症患者之再測信度、隨機測量誤差及練習效應。研究者採用配對t檢定檢驗患者初測和再測於iSAT低(高)負載和低(高)負載含干擾物情境的分數有無顯著差異,以確定iSAT之建構效度。同時,研究者以患者之蒙特利爾智能測驗 (Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA)分數檢驗iSAT之收斂效度,並以患者的個人與社會功能量表分數檢驗iSAT之生態效度。此外,研究者以MoCA 26分為效標,檢驗iSAT是否可區辨思覺失調症二個年齡族群(年輕[20至39歲]及中老年[40歲以上])中整體認知功能好(MoCA≧26)及差(MoCA<26)之患者,以檢驗其區辨效度。研究三為iSAT於20至29歲年輕成人之心理計量特性驗證,作為未來常模建立之參考。研究者募集20至29歲年輕成人參與二次iSAT評估(間隔二週),以檢驗iSAT應用於20至29歲年輕成人之再測信度、隨機測量誤差及練習效應,並檢驗二次iSAT分數平均為樣本前25%的受試者與後25%的受試者的分數是否有顯著差異,以驗證區辨效度。研究者亦採用配對t檢定檢驗患者於iSAT低(高)負載和低(高)負載含干擾物情境的分數有無顯著差異,以確定iSAT之建構效度。同時,研究者亦以平板電腦版符號數字轉換測驗檢驗iSAT之收斂效度。 結果:研究者透過5位專家對iSAT內容設計、施測方式及計分之審核,和10位20至29歲年輕成人及10位20歲以上思覺失調症患者對iSAT內容之迴饋,建立iSAT之內容效度和確定其可行性。iSAT應用於思覺失調症患者具備建構效度、適用於研究之再測信度,以及良好之收斂效度。iSAT二種低負載情境測驗具備區辨效度、可接受之隨機測量誤差和微小可忽略之練習效應,但二種高負載情境測驗不具備區辨效度,且隨機測量誤差較大,練習效應亦為低至中度。iSAT之生態效度亦不佳。另外,iSAT應用於20至29歲年輕成人具備區辨效度及中度之再測信度,二種低負載情境具備可接受之隨機測量誤差,但其練習效應為低至中度,而二種高負載情境之隨機測量誤差略大,且練習效應為中至高度。此外,iSAT於20至29歲年輕成人不具備建構效度,其收斂效度亦不佳。 結論:iSAT之心理計量特性適用於思覺失調症患者之研究,但研究者建議使用者以多次評量和平均分數降低iSAT二種高負載情境之隨機測量誤差,以提昇其區辨效度。目前iSAT之心理計量特性尚不足以建立常模。未來研究者可納入不同地區、慣用手為左手及教育年數為12至16年間之20至29歲年輕成人為樣本,再次驗證iSAT於20至29歲年輕成人之心理計量特性,便於日後建立常模。 | zh_TW |
dc.description.abstract | Background and purpose: Selective attention deficit is common in patients with schizophrenia. Such deficit can affect their functional performance, so assessing selective attention in patients with schizophrenia is essential. Perceptual load theory considers the effects of the perceptual load of a task on the selectivity mechanism of attention. With the evidence of neurophysiology, perceptual load theory provides the clearest explanation of the mechanism of selective attention. However, no tests of selective attention have been developed on the basis of perceptual load theory, so clinicians are unable to provide a thorough evaluation to determine patients’ problems in selective attention. Therefore, this dissertation had three purposes: First, to develop an iPad-based Selective Attention test (iSAT) on the basis of perceptual load theory, to establish its content validity, and to evaluate its feasibility; second, to examine the psychometric properties of the iSAT in patients with schizophrenia so as to improve its utility in clinical and research settings; and third, to examine the psychometric properties of the iSAT in young adults aged 20 to 29 years so as to ensure appropriate psychometric properties for the development of future norms.
Methods: This dissertation consists of 3 studies. Study 1: Development of the iSAT and determination of the content validity and feasibility of the iSAT. This study was conducted in 3 stages: (1) development of the iSAT’s first version and mobile applications on the basis of perceptual load theory and paradigm as well as expert opinion; (2) recruitment of young adults aged 20 to 29 years and schizophrenia patients aged ≧20 years to test the feasibility of the iSAT’s first version and then develop the iSAT’s second version according to the examinees’ suggestions and the experts’ opinions; (3) recruitment of young adults aged 20 to 29 years and patients with schizophrenia aged ≧20 years to test the feasibility of the iSAT’s second version so as to determine the content of the iSAT’s formal version and its content validity. Study 2: Validation of psychometric properties of the iSAT in patients with schizophrenia. The researcher recruited schizophrenia patients aged ≧20 years to be assessed twice by the iSAT (2 weeks apart) to examine the test-retest reliability, random measurement error, and practice effect of the iSAT. Paired t tests were also used to examine whether the patients’ scores of the low (high) load condition and those of the low (high) load with distractor condition were significantly different so as to determine the construct validity. Moreover, the scores of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) and the Personal and Social Performance scale were used to examine the convergent validity and ecological validity, respectively. A MoCA score of ≧26 was used as a criterion to evaluate whether the iSAT can discriminate two age groups in patients with schizophrenia [young (20 to 39 years) and middle aged and elderly (≧40 years)] to determine the discriminative validity. Study 3: Validation of the psychometric properties of the iSAT in young adults aged 20 to 29 years to develop future norms. The researcher recruited young adults aged 20 to 29 years to be assessed twice by the iSAT (2 weeks apart) to examine the test-retest reliability, random measurement error, and practice effect. To examine the discriminative validity, the researcher examined whether the highest 25% and lowest 25% of the iSAT average scores (mean of test and retest) were significantly different. To examine the construct validity, paired t tests were used to examine whether the scores of the low (high) load condition and those of the low (high) load with distractor condition were significantly different. Moreover, the scores of the Tablet-based Symbol Digit Modalities Test were used to examine the convergent validity. Results: The content validity and feasibility of the iSAT have been confirmed by five experts’ examinations regarding its design, administration and scoring as well as the feedback from 10 young adults aged 20 to 29 years and 10 schizophrenia patients aged ≧20 years. The iSAT had construct validity, sufficient test-retest reliability for research use, and good convergent validity in patients with schizophrenia. The two low-load conditions had discriminative validity, acceptable random measurement, and a trivial practice effect. However, the two high-load conditions did not have discriminative validity. The two high-load conditions had substantial random measurement error and a low to moderate practice effect. The ecological validity of the iSAT was insufficient. As for young adults aged 20 to 29 years, the iSAT had discriminative validity and moderate test-retest reliability. The two low-load conditions had acceptable random measurement error but a low to moderate practice effect. The two high-load conditions had slightly larger random measurement error as well as a moderate to high practice effect. Moreover, the iSAT did not have construct validity, and its convergent validity was insufficient in young adults aged 20 to 29 years. Conclusion: The psychometric properties of the iSAT are satisfactory for conducting research in patients with schizophrenia. However, the researcher suggests that prospective users use multiple administrations and average scores to lower the random measurement error found in the two high-load conditions of the iSAT and thereby to improve the discriminative validity. On the other hand, the psychometric properties of the iSAT are not sufficient to develop norms. Future studies may recruit young adults aged 20 to 29 years who are from different areas, left-handed, and have 12 to 16 years of schooling as study subjects to re-examine the psychometric properties of the iSAT in young adults aged 20 to 29 years for the development of future norms. | en |
dc.description.provenance | Made available in DSpace on 2021-05-12T09:36:16Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1 ntu-107-D00429001-1.pdf: 5751748 bytes, checksum: a08ff37545a233c438dad0120bce5c94 (MD5) Previous issue date: 2018 | en |
dc.description.tableofcontents | 目錄
誌謝………………………………………………….……………………...i 中文摘要………………………………………………….………………..ii Abstract………………………………………………….…………...........iv 縮寫對照表………………………………………………….…...............xiv 第一章、注意力簡介……………………………………………………...1 第一節、注意力之定義…………………………………………….1 第二節、注意力之三個主要特性及其重要性…………………….2 第三節、結論……………………………………………………….2 第二章、選擇性注意力之不同理論概念……………………………….3 第一節、認知心理學觀點………………………………………….3 一、瓶頸理論……………………………………………………….3 二、容量理論……………………………………………………….7 三、多元理論……………………………………………………….8 第二節、神經心理學觀點………………………………………….8 一、注意力網絡模式……………………………………………….9 二、Mirsky注意力模式…………………………………………...10 三、Cohen注意力模式……………………………………………10 四、Van Zomeren和Brouwer注意力模式……………………….11 第三節、臨床觀點…………………………………………………12 一、注意力臨床模式………………………………………………12 第四節、各學派注意力理論之評析………………………………12 第五節、結論………………………………………………………13 第三章、思覺失調症患者選擇性注意力缺失及其功能性影響……….14 第一節、思覺失調症之簡介………………………………………14 一、思覺失調症之名稱由來……………………………………..14 二、思覺失調症之病因…………………………………………..14 三、思覺失調症之流行病學……………………………………..15 四、思覺失調症之臨床症狀……………………………………..15 第二節、思覺失調症患者選擇性注意力缺失及其功能性影響....16 第三節、結論………………………………………………………17 第四章、常用於思覺失調症患者之選擇性注意力測驗介紹與評析….19 第一節、常用於思覺失調症患者之選擇性注意力測驗介紹與評析……………………………………………………………………19 一、認知心理學觀點:注意力歷程自動化與否之相關測驗…..19 二、神經心理學觀點:反應抑制之相關測驗…………………..22 三、神經心理學觀點:注意力系統的網絡特性之相關測驗…..24 第二節、結論………………………………………………………27 第五章、選擇性注意力測驗須具備之心理計量特性及其臨床與研究意 義………………………………………………………………………...29 第一節、再測信度…………………………………………………29 第二節、隨機測量誤差……………………………………………30 第三節、練習效應…………………………………………………31 第四節、內容效度…………………………………………………32 第五節、建構效度…………………………………………………33 第六節、收斂效度…………………………………………………34 第七節、區辨效度…………………………………………………35 第八節、生態效度…………………………………………………35 第九節、結論………………………………………………………36 第六章、常模之重要性及建立常模前須具備之條件………………….38 第一節、常模之重要性……………………………………………38 第二節、建立常模前須具備之條件………………………………38 第七章、研究動機與目的……………………………………………….39 第八章、研究一:iPad版選擇性注意力測驗之發展………………….41 研究目的……………………………………………………………41 方法…………………………………………………………………41 結果…………………………………………………………………43 討論…………………………………………………………………47 結論…………………………………………………………………48 第九章、研究二:iPad版選擇性注意力測驗於思覺失調症患者之心理計量特性驗證…………………………………………………………...49 研究目的……………………………………………………………49 方法…………………………………………………………………49 結果…………………………………………………………………54 討論…………………………………………………………………57 結論…………………………………………………………………61 第十章、研究三:iPad版選擇性注意力測驗於20至29歲年輕成人之心理計量特性驗證……………………………………………………...63 研究目的……………………………………………………………63 方法…………………………………………………………………63 結果…………………………………………………………………66 討論…………………………………………………………………68 結論…………………………………………………………………72 第十一章、總結、測驗修改及未來研究方向…………………………..73 第一節、總結………………………………………………………73 第二節、測驗修改及未來研究方向……………………………...73 參考文獻………………………………………………………………...76 表目錄 表一、研究一20至29歲年輕成人之人口學資料…………………….93 表二、研究一思覺失調症患者之人口學與病情資料………………….94 表三、研究二思覺失調症患者之人口學與病情資料………………….95 表四、iSAT於思覺失調症患者之再測信度、隨機測量誤差和練習效應………………………………………………………………………….96 表五、iSAT於思覺失調症患者之收斂效度與生態效度……………....97 表六、年輕組和中老年組整體認知功能好(MoCA≧26)與差(MoCA<26)之思覺失調症患者於四種測驗情境之iSAT分數比較…………………98 表七、研究三20至29 歲年輕成人之人口學資料……………………99 表八、iSAT於20至29歲年輕成人之再測信度、隨機測量誤差和練習應...………………………………………………………………............100 表九、20至29歲年輕成人樣本前25%、50%和後25%之iSAT分數比較.. …........................................................................................................101 圖目錄 圖一、過濾器模式…………………………………………………….102 圖二、雙耳分聽實驗………………………………………………….103 圖三、減弱模式……………………………………………………….104 圖四、記憶選擇模式……….………………………………………....105 圖五、知覺負載理論實驗典範─低負載含干擾物情境題……….....106 圖六、知覺負載理論實驗典範─高負載含干擾物情境題………….107 圖七、自動式察覺…………………………………………………….108 圖八、控制式搜尋…………………………………………………….109 圖九、SAAT之測驗內容……………………………………………..110 圖十、SAAT刺激呈現之流程………………………………………..111 圖十一、SCWT相關實驗內容範例………………………………….112 圖十二、Posner線索典範之範例…………………………………….113 圖十三、旁側抑制作業……………………………………………….114 圖十四、注意力網絡測驗之內容…………………………………….115 圖十五、研究架構圖………………………………………………….116 圖十六、iSAT之擺設…………………………………………………117 圖十七、iSAT之低負載情境…………………………………………118 圖十八、iSAT之低負載含干擾物情境………………………………119 圖十九、iSAT之高負載情境…………………………………………120 圖二十、iSAT之高負載含干擾物情境………………………………..121 圖二十一、研究二之流程……………………………………………..122 附錄目錄 附錄一、iPad版選擇性注意力測驗施測手冊………………………..123 附錄二、臨床整體印象-疾病嚴重性量表……………………………..139 附錄三、蒙特利爾智能測驗…………………………………………..140 附錄四、個人與社會功能量表………………………………………..141 附錄五、平板電腦版符號數字轉換測驗施測手冊…………………..144 | |
dc.language.iso | zh-TW | |
dc.title | 發展與驗證iPad版選擇性注意力測驗於思覺失調症患者之心理計量特性 | zh_TW |
dc.title | Development and Validation of an iPad-based Selective Attention Test in Patients with Schizophrenia | en |
dc.type | Thesis | |
dc.date.schoolyear | 106-1 | |
dc.description.degree | 博士 | |
dc.contributor.oralexamcommittee | 謝清麟,林鈜宇,劉震鐘,吳建德 | |
dc.subject.keyword | 選擇性注意力,知覺負載理論,思覺失調症,成人,心理計量特性, | zh_TW |
dc.subject.keyword | selective attention,perceptual load theory,schizophrenia,adult,psychometric properties, | en |
dc.relation.page | 154 | |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.6342/NTU201800034 | |
dc.rights.note | 同意授權(全球公開) | |
dc.date.accepted | 2018-01-12 | |
dc.contributor.author-college | 醫學院 | zh_TW |
dc.contributor.author-dept | 職能治療研究所 | zh_TW |
顯示於系所單位: | 職能治療學系 |
文件中的檔案:
檔案 | 大小 | 格式 | |
---|---|---|---|
ntu-107-1.pdf | 5.62 MB | Adobe PDF | 檢視/開啟 |
系統中的文件,除了特別指名其著作權條款之外,均受到著作權保護,並且保留所有的權利。