Skip navigation

DSpace

機構典藏 DSpace 系統致力於保存各式數位資料(如:文字、圖片、PDF)並使其易於取用。

點此認識 DSpace
DSpace logo
English
中文
  • 瀏覽論文
    • 校院系所
    • 出版年
    • 作者
    • 標題
    • 關鍵字
  • 搜尋 TDR
  • 授權 Q&A
    • 我的頁面
    • 接受 E-mail 通知
    • 編輯個人資料
  1. NTU Theses and Dissertations Repository
  2. 理學院
  3. 心理學系
請用此 Handle URI 來引用此文件: http://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/91931
完整後設資料紀錄
DC 欄位值語言
dc.contributor.advisor李怡青zh_TW
dc.contributor.advisorI-Ching Leeen
dc.contributor.author陳力瑜zh_TW
dc.contributor.authorLi-Yu Chenen
dc.date.accessioned2024-02-26T16:30:37Z-
dc.date.available2024-02-27-
dc.date.copyright2024-02-26-
dc.date.issued2022-
dc.date.submitted2002-01-01-
dc.identifier.citation李亦園(1978):《信仰與文化》(初版)。台北:巨流圖書公司。[Lee, Y.Y. (1978). Faith and Culture (1st ed.). Taipei: Chu-Liu Publisher.]
楊國樞(1981):〈中國人的性格與行為:形成及蛻變〉。《中華心理學刊》,23,39-55。[Yang, K.S. (1981). Chinese character and behavior: Formation and transformation. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 23, 39-55.]
楊國樞(1993):〈中國人的社會取向:社會互動的觀點〉。見楊國樞、余安邦(編):《中國人的心理與行為──理念及方法篇(一九九二)》,頁131-142。台北:桂冠圖書公司。[Yang, K.S. (1993). Chinese social orientation: A social interaction perspective. In K.S.Yang & A. B. Yu (1st ed.) Chinese people's psychology and behavior: Philosophy and methods (pp. 131-142). Taipei: Laureate Publisher.]
楊國樞、葉明華(2005):〈家族主義與泛家族主義〉。見楊國樞、黃光國、楊中芳(編):《華人本土心理學》,頁250-292。台北:遠流。[Yang, K.S. & Yeh, M.H. (2005). Familism and pan-familism. In K. S. Yang, K. K. Huang & C. F. Yang (Eds.) Psychology in Chinese societies (pp. 250-292). Taipei: Yuan-Liou Publishing Co., Ltd.]
楊懋舂(1972):〈中國的家族主義與國民性格〉。見李亦園、楊國樞(編):《中國人的性格》,頁106-145。台北:中央研究院民族學研究所。[Yang, M.C. (1972). Familism and national character in China. In Y. Y. Lee & K. S. Yang (Eds.) Chinese people's personality (pp. 106-145). Taipei: Institute of Ethnology, Academia Sinica.]
葉明華、楊國樞(1998):〈中國人的家族主義:概念分析與實徵研究〉。《中央研究院民族學研究所集刊》,83,169-225。[Yeh, M.H. & Yang, K.S. (1998). Chinese familism: Conceptual analysis and empirical research. Bulletin of the Institute of Ethnology Academia Sinica, 83, 169-225.]
謝繼昌(1982):〈中國家族研究的探討〉。見楊國樞、文崇一(編):《社會及行為科學研究的中國化》,頁255-280。台北:中央研究院民族學研究所。[Hsieh, J.C. (1982). A probe into Chinese family studies. In K. S. Yang & C. Y. Weng(Ed.) Sinicization of social and behavioral scientific research (pp. 255-280). Taipei: Institute of Ethnology, Academia Sinica.]
Altemeyer, B. (1983). Right-wing authoritarianism. Univ. of Manitoba Press.
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(4), 596-612. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy qquotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning aautism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 163-175. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:Jadd.0000022607.19833.00
Batson, C. D. (2018). A scientific search for altruism: Do we only care about ourselves? Oxford University Press.
Batson, C. D., Chang, J., Orr, R., & Rowland, J. (2002). Empathy, attitudes, and action: Can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group motivate one to help the group? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(12), 1656-1666.
Benz, M., & Meier, S. (2008). Do people behave in experiments as in the field?—evidence from donations. Experimental Economics, 11(3), 268-281.
Biernat, M., Vescio, T. K., & Billings, L. S. (1999). Black sheep and expectancy violation: Integrating two models of social judgment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29(4), 523-542.
Billig, M., & Tajfel, H. (1973). Social categorization and similarity in intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 3(1), 27-52.
Brewer, M. B., & Brown, R. J. (1998). Intergroup relations. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 554–594). New York: McGrawHill.
Calderón-Tena, C. O., Knight, G. P., & Carlo, G. (2011). The socialization of prosocial behavioral tendencies among Mexican American adolescents: The role of familism values. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 17(1), 98.
Chae, J., Kim, K., Kim, Y., Lim, G., Kim, D., & Kim, H. (2022). Ingroup favoritism overrides fairness when resources are limited. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 1-11.
Cheung, F. M. (1990). People against the mentally ill: Community opposition to residential treatment facilities. Community Mental Health Journal, 26(2), 205-212.
Cohen, J., & Struening, E. L. (1962). Opinions about mental illness in the personnel of two large mental hospitals. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64(5), 349.
Corona, K., Campos, B., & Chen, C. (2017). Familism is associated with psychological well-being and physical health: Main effects and stress-buffering effects. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 39(1), 46-65.
Corrigan, P. W., & Watson, A. C. (2003). Factors that explain how policy makers distribute resources to mental health services. Psychiatric Services, 54(4), 501-507.
Dai, B., Mao, Z., Wu, B., Mei, Y. J., Levkoff, S., & Wang, H. (2015). Family caregiver's perception of Alzheimer's disease and caregiving in Chinese culture. Social Work in Public Health, 30(2), 185-196. https://doi.org/10.1080/19371918.2014.969858
de Waal, F. B. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279-300. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625
Decety, J., Bartal, I. B.-A., Uzefovsky, F., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2016). Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1686), 20150077.
Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., Schroeder, D. A., & Penner, L. A. (2017). The social psychology of prosocial behavior. Psychology Press.
Eisenberg, N., Eggum, N. D., & Di Giunta, L. (2010). Empathy-related Responding: Associations with Prosocial Behavior, Aggression, and Intergroup Relations. Social Issues and Policy Review, 4(1), 143-180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-2409.2010.01020.x
Feldman, S., Huddy, L., Wronski, J., & Lown, P. (2016). Compassionate policy support: The interplay of empathy and ideology. International Society of Political Psychology Conference
Feldman, S., Huddy, L., Wronski, J., & Lown, P. (2020). The interplay of empathy and individualism in support for social welfare policies. Political Psychology, 41(2), 343-362. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12620
Feldman, S., & Steenbergen, M. R. (2001). The humanitarian foundation of public support for social welfare. American Journal of Political Science, 658-677.
Feldman, S., & Zaller, J. (1992). The political culture of ambivalence: Ideological responses to the welfare state. American Journal of Political Science, 268-307.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance (Vol. 2). Stanford university press.
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2014). Reducing intergroup bias: The common ingroup identity model. Psychology Press.
Gilens, M. (1995). Racial attitudes and opposition to welfare. The Journal of Politics, 57(4), 994-1014.
Goette, L., Huffman, D., & Meier, S. (2006). The impact of group membership on cooperation and norm enforcement: Evidence using random assignment to real social groups. American Economic Review, 96(2), 212-216.
Guan, R., Jin, L., & Qian, M. (2012). Validation of the empathy quotient–short form among Chinese healthcare professionals. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 40(1), 75-84.
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 17-52. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-5193(64)90039-6
Hogg, M. A. (2001). A social identity theory of leadership. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(3), 184-200.
Hoshino‐Browne, E. (2012). Cultural variations in motivation for cognitive consistency: Influences of self‐systems on cognitive dissonance. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6(2), 126-141.
Hsu, F. L. (1953). Americans and Chinese: Two Ways of Life (1st ed.). New York: Henry Schuman.
Hsu, J. (1985). The Chinese family: Relations, problems, and therapy. In W. Tseng and D. Y. H. Wu (Eds.), Chinese culture and mental health (pp. 95-112). Elsevier.
Jost, J. T. (2006). The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist, 61(7), 651-670. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.651
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Sulloway, F. J., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2018). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Routledge.
Katz, D., & Stotland, E. (1959). A preliminary statement to a theory of attitude structure and change. Psychology: A study of a Science, 3(423-475).
Kurland, J. A., & Gaulin, S. J. (2015). Cooperation and conflict among kin. The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, 447-482.
Lai, F. H., Siu, A. M., & Shek, D. T. (2015). Individual and social predictors of prosocial behavior among Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. Frontiers in Pediatrics, 3, 39.
Lawrence, E. J., Shaw, P., Baker, D., Baron-Cohen, S., & David, A. S. (2004). Measuring empathy: reliability and validity of the Empathy Quotient. Psychological Medicine, 34(5), 911-919. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291703001624
Lee, I. C., Permyakova, T., & Sheveleva, M. (2020). Reducing negative attitudes toward immigrants in Russia and Taiwan: Possible beneficial effects of naive dialecticism and an incremental worldview. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 572386. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.572386
Levine, M., Prosser, A., Evans, D., & Reicher, S. (2005). Identity and emergency intervention: How social group membership and inclusiveness of group boundaries shape helping behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(4), 443-453.
Lewis, A. C., & Sherman, S. J. (2003). Hiring you makes me look bad: Social-identity based reversals of the ingroup favoritism effect. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 90(2), 262-276.
Li, J., Han, Z. R., Gao, M. M., Sun, X., & Ahemaitijiang, N. (2018). Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS): Factor structure, reliability, and validity. Psychological Assessment, 30(5), e1-e9. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000582
Li, Y., & Zhao, M. (2019). Effects of the presence of others on prosocial behavior: Perceived face as mediator. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 22(2), 193-202.
Luengo Kanacri, B. P., Eisenberg, N., Tramontano, C., Zuffiano, A., Caprara, M. G., Regner, E., Zhu, L., Pastorelli, C., & Caprara, G. V. (2021). Measuring prosocial behaviors: Psychometric properties and cross-national validation of the prosociality scale in five countries. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 693174. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.693174
Méndez, J. E., & Mendez, J. E. (2013). Report of the Special Rapporteur on torture and other cruel, inhuman or degrading treatment or punishment. Human Rights Council.
Mahomed, F. (2020). Addressing the problem of severe underinvestment in mental health and well-being from a human rights perspective. Health Hum Rights, 22(1), 35-49. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32669787
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2010). Cultures and selves: A cycle of mutual constitution. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 420-430. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610375557
Marques, J. M., & Paez, D. (1994). The ‘black sheep effect’: Social categorization, rejection of ingroup deviates, and perception of group variability. European Review of Social Psychology, 5(1), 37-68.
McFarland, S., Webb, M., & Brown, D. (2012). All humanity is my ingroup: a measure and studies of identification with all humanity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(5), 830.
Messick, D. M., & Mackie, D. M. (1989). Intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 45–81
Mesurado, B., Guerra, P., Richaud, M. C., & Rodriguez, L. M. (2019). Effectiveness of prosocial behavior interventions: A meta-analysis. In P. Á. Gargiulo and H. L. Mesones Arroyo (Eds.), Psychiatry and neuroscience update (pp. 259-271). Springer.
Petersen, M. B., Slothuus, R., Stubager, R., & Togeby, L. (2011). Deservingness versus values in public opinion on welfare: The automaticity of the deservingness heuristic. European Journal of Political Research, 50(1), 24-52.
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(4), 741.
Reed II, A., & Aquino, K. F. (2003). Moral identity and the expanding circle of moral regard toward out-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(6), 1270.
Shiga, S. (1978). Family property and the law of inheritance in traditional China. In D. C. Buxbaum (Ed.), Chinese family law and social change in historical and comparative perspective (pp. 109-150). Seattle and London: University of Washington Press.
Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1(2), 149-178.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (2004). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In J. T. Jost and J. Sidanius (Eds.), Political psychology (pp. 276-293). Psychology Press.
Taylor, S. M., & Dear, M. J. (1981). Scaling community attitudes toward the mentally ill. Schizophr Bull, 7(2), 225-240. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/7.2.225
Thielmann, I., Spadaro, G., & Balliet, D. (2020). Personality and prosocial behavior: A theoretical framework and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(1), 30-90. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000217
Turner, J. C., Brown, R. J., & Tajfel, H. (1979). Social comparison and group interest in ingroup favouritism. European Journal of Social Psychology, 9(2), 187-204.
Van Lange, P. A., Bekkers, R., Chirumbolo, A., & Leone, L. (2012). Are conservatives less likely to be prosocial than liberals? From games to ideology, political preferences and votingg. European Journal of Personality, 26(5), 461-473.
Wang, Y.-g., Wang, Y.-q., Chen, S.-l., Zhu, C.-y., & Wang, K. (2008). Theory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: A componential view. Psychiatry Research, 161(2), 153-161.
Whitt, S., & Wilson, R. K. (2007). The dictator game, fairness and ethnicity in postwar Bosnia. American Journal of Political Science, 51(3), 655-668.
Yang, C.-f. (1988). Familism and development: An examination of the role of family in contemporary China mainland, Hong Kong and Taiwan. In D. Sinha & H. S. R. Kao (Eds.), Social values and development: Asian perspectives (pp. 93-123). Sage Publications, Inc.
Yang, K.-S. (1993). Chinese social orientation: An integrative analysis. In L. Y. Cheng, F. M. C. Cheung, and C. N. Chen (Eds.), Psychotherapy for the Chinese: Selected papers from the first international conference (pp. 19- 56). Hong Kong: Chinese University of Hong Kong
Zettler, I., & Hilbig, B. E. (2010). Attitudes of the selfless: Explaining political orientation with altruism. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(3), 338-342.
Zettler, I., Hilbig, B. E., & Haubrich, J. (2011). Altruism at the ballots: Predicting political attitudes and behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(1), 130-133.
-
dc.identifier.urihttp://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/91931-
dc.description.abstract精神醫療資源的缺乏是各國普遍且嚴峻面對的議題,其中一個關鍵的原因在於人們對於將資源分配給精神病患的反對態度,瞭解人們挹注資源之意願背後的潛在機制將可能為我們帶來解方。過去的研究發現,保守派的個人主義信念會調節同理能力與社福政策支持意願的關係,相比於低同理能力的保守派,同理能力較高的保守派反而更反對挹注社福資源予有困難的人,對於這些人的處境也更加漠不關心;然而,當資源的來源是慈善團體而非政府之社福政策時,則沒有這個現象(Feldman, Huddy, Wronski, & Lown, 2020),這樣特殊的同理能力、保守意識形態、與資源來源的交互關係是否亦存在於華人社會之中仍有待探討。此外,除了同理、意識形態、與資源來源,內外群體之別也可能是重要的影響因子,華人的家族主義文化可能使得人們特別優待以親緣為基礎所形成的內團體。我們透過三個研究來探討以上這些因素如何影響台灣人對於精神疾病患者的幫助意願。研究一的參與者(有效樣本271人)被隨機分派至四種補助精神病患之情境,以評估他們支持幫助與同情關懷的程度,在其中三個情境中的病患分別是陌生人、與參與者沒有太多互動的親人(親緣為基礎之內群體成員)及同所大學的人(類別為基礎之內群體成員),補助的資金則來自於政府的社會福利政策,第四個情境中的補助資金源於慈善團體捐助,受助病患則是陌生人。研究二則隨機指派參與者(有效樣本311人)至八組情境,探討血緣(親人或非親人)、關係品質(普通或交惡)、與資金來源(政府或慈善)的影響。研究三(有效樣本373人)將研究二的關係品質操弄修改為關係品質良好與交惡的情境。研究結果未能重複驗證Feldman等人所發現的同理、意識形態、與資金來源的交互效果,即便是那些自陳具有個人主義傾向的參與者也沒有此趨勢。然而,我們發現,當人們想像自己有親人是精神病患時,會更視受助對象為內群體成員,而有較高的資源分配意願與同情關懷的程度。且這個效果不受到關係品質的影響,即便是關係交惡的親人,人們仍然有較高的幫助意願。我們進一步討論了研究結果的意義與貢獻。zh_TW
dc.description.abstractUnder-resourced mental healthcare is severe and prevalent worldwide. One critical cause is the reluctance to allocate resources to mental patients. Understanding the underlying mechanism behind people's (un)willingness to help mental patients can shed light on the potential solutions. Past research has found that individualistic conservative ideology moderates the relationship between empathy and welfare support toward people in need, in which the higher empathic ability the conservatives have, the lower welfare support they showed for those in need (but no such moderation when the resources were from charity donations, Feldman, Huddy, Wronski, & Lown, 2020). Whether this empathy, funding sources, and conservative ideology interaction exists in Chinese societies has not been investigated. In addition to empathy, funding sources, and conservative ideology, people may respond to kinship-based ingroup differently in Chinese societies. Through three survey experiments, we investigated how these factors affect Taiwanese people's prosociality toward mental patients. In Study 1, participants (N = 279) were randomly assigned to four conditions by reading a scenario of an imaginary mental patient who needed resources from either government or charity funds. In the government funds conditions, the patient was either a relative of the participants (kinship-based ingroup without much interaction), a student from the same college as the participant (category-based ingroup without much interaction), or a stranger. In the charity funds condition, the patient was a stranger. In Study 2, participants (N = 311) were randomly assigned to eight conditions: 2 (kinship vs. non-kinship) x 2 (government vs. charity funds) x 2 (without much interaction vs. bad relationships). In Study 3 (N = 373), the relationship manipulation was modified (bad relationships vs. good relationships) to replace the relationship manipulation in Study 2. After reading the assigned scenario, participants were asked about their support for assistance on mental patients and their compassion for mental patients. Our results did not replicate Feldman and his colleagues’ findings, not even among those who self-evaluated themselves to be individualistic. However, we found that participants were more likely to support mental patients when the target was kin, regardless of the relationship quality between the target and the participants. Implications regarding the underlying mechanism behind people's (un)willingness to help mental patients through resource allocations were further discussed.en
dc.description.provenanceSubmitted by admin ntu (admin@lib.ntu.edu.tw) on 2024-02-26T16:30:37Z
No. of bitstreams: 0
en
dc.description.provenanceMade available in DSpace on 2024-02-26T16:30:37Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 0en
dc.description.tableofcontentsAcknowledgement ……………………………………………………………….. i
Chinese Abstract …………………………………………………………………...…ii
English Abstract………………………………………………………………...……iv
1. Introduction …......………………………………………………………….... 1
2. Pilot Study ....……….….………………………………………………....... 12
2.1 Method .……………………...…………………………………...... 12
2.2 Results ...............……………………..……………………....…....... 14
3. Study 1 ……..….…….……………………………………………………... 16
3.1 Method .……………………...…………………………………….. 17
3.2 Results ...............……………………..…………………………….. 23
4. Study 2 ……..….…….……………………………………………………... 31
4.1 Method .……………………...…………………………………….. 31
4.2 Results ...............……………………..…………………………….. 36
5. Study 3 ……..….…….……………………………………………………... 44
5.1 Method .……………………...…………………………………….. 44
5.2 Results ...............……………………..…………………………….. 46
6. General discussion …………...……………………………...………….. 50
7. References ……………………………………………………………... 56
Appendix …………………………………………………………………..... 69
-
dc.language.isoen-
dc.title家人就是不一樣:親屬關係想像增進幫助精神病患之意願zh_TW
dc.titleKinship in Name Only is Powerful: The Effect of Kinship Imagination on Helping People with Mental Illnessen
dc.typeThesis-
dc.date.schoolyear110-2-
dc.description.degree碩士-
dc.contributor.oralexamcommittee連盈如;林耀盛zh_TW
dc.contributor.oralexamcommitteeYin-Ju Lien;Yaw-Sheng Linen
dc.subject.keyword家族主義,資源分配,社會福利政策,精神醫療,zh_TW
dc.subject.keywordkinship,resource allocation,welfare policies,mental health care,en
dc.relation.page74-
dc.identifier.doi10.6342/NTU202203746-
dc.rights.note同意授權(限校園內公開)-
dc.date.accepted2022-09-27-
dc.contributor.author-college理學院-
dc.contributor.author-dept心理學系-
顯示於系所單位:心理學系

文件中的檔案:
檔案 大小格式 
ntu-110-2.pdf
授權僅限NTU校內IP使用(校園外請利用VPN校外連線服務)
1.22 MBAdobe PDF檢視/開啟
顯示文件簡單紀錄


系統中的文件,除了特別指名其著作權條款之外,均受到著作權保護,並且保留所有的權利。

社群連結
聯絡資訊
10617臺北市大安區羅斯福路四段1號
No.1 Sec.4, Roosevelt Rd., Taipei, Taiwan, R.O.C. 106
Tel: (02)33662353
Email: ntuetds@ntu.edu.tw
意見箱
相關連結
館藏目錄
國內圖書館整合查詢 MetaCat
臺大學術典藏 NTU Scholars
臺大圖書館數位典藏館
本站聲明
© NTU Library All Rights Reserved