Skip navigation

DSpace

機構典藏 DSpace 系統致力於保存各式數位資料(如:文字、圖片、PDF)並使其易於取用。

點此認識 DSpace
DSpace logo
English
中文
  • 瀏覽論文
    • 校院系所
    • 出版年
    • 作者
    • 標題
    • 關鍵字
  • 搜尋 TDR
  • 授權 Q&A
    • 我的頁面
    • 接受 E-mail 通知
    • 編輯個人資料
  1. NTU Theses and Dissertations Repository
  2. 理學院
  3. 心理學系
請用此 Handle URI 來引用此文件: http://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/83172
完整後設資料紀錄
DC 欄位值語言
dc.contributor.advisor謝伯讓zh_TW
dc.contributor.advisorPo-Jang Hsiehen
dc.contributor.author陳宇安zh_TW
dc.contributor.authorYu-An Chenen
dc.date.accessioned2023-01-10T17:07:20Z-
dc.date.available2023-11-09-
dc.date.copyright2023-01-07-
dc.date.issued2022-
dc.date.submitted2022-12-30-
dc.identifier.citationAlperin, B. R., Christoff, K., Mills, C., & Karalunas, S. L. (2021). More than off-task: Increased freely-moving thought in ADHD. Consciousness and Cognition, 93, 103156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2021.103156
Charles, L., Chardin, C., & Haggard, P. (2020). Evidence for metacognitive bias in perception of voluntary action. Cognition, 194, 104041. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104041
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 17(11), 718–731. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.113
Drescher, L. H., Bussche, E. V. den, & Desender, K. (2018). Absence without leave or leave without absence: Examining the interrelations among mind wandering, metacognition and cognitive control. PLOS ONE, 13(2), e0191639. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191639
Ekwaru, J. P., & Veugelers, P. J. (2018). The overlooked importance of constants added in log transformation of independent variables with zero values: A proposed approach for determining an optimal constant. Statistics in Biopharmaceutical Research, 10(1), 26–29. https://doi.org/10.1080/19466315.2017.1369900
Fleming, S. M. (2017). HMeta-d: hierarchical Bayesian estimation of metacognitive efficiency from confidence ratings. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 3(1), nix007-. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/nix007
Fleming, S. M., & Lau, H. C. (2014). How to measure metacognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 443. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00443
Fleming, S. M., Ryu, J., Golfinos, J. G., & Blackmon, K. E. (2014). Domain-specific impairment in metacognitive accuracy following anterior prefrontal lesions. Brain, 137(10), 2811–2822. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awu221
Giambra, L. M. (1989). Task-unrelated-thought frequency as a function of age: A laboratory study. Psychology and Aging, 4(2), 136–143. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.4.2.136
Hayes, A. F. (2022). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Head, J., & Helton, W. S. (2018). The troubling science of neurophenomenology. Experimental Brain Research, 236(9), 2463–2467. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4623-7
Irving, Z. C., & Glasser, A. (2019). Mind‐wandering: A philosophical guide. Philosophy Compass, 15(1). https://doi.org/10.1111/phc3.12644
Ju, Y.-J., & Lien, Y.-W. (2018). Who is prone to wander and when? Examining an integrative effect of working memory capacity and mindfulness trait on mind wandering under different task loads. Consciousness and Cognition, 63, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.06.006
Kam, J. W. Y., Irving, Z. C., Mills, C., Patel, S., Gopnik, A., & Knight, R. T. (2021). Distinct electrophysiological signatures of task-unrelated and dynamic thoughts. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(4), e2011796118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2011796118
Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: An experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychological Science, 18(7), 614–621. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01948.x
Kane, M. J., Smeekens, B. A., Meier, M. E., Welhaf, M. S., & Phillips, N. E. (2021). Testing the construct validity of competing measurement approaches to probed mind-wandering reports. Behavior Research Methods, 1–40. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01557-x
Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330(6006), 932–932. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1192439
Koch, C., Massimini, M., Boly, M., & Tononi, G. (2016). Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(5), 307–321. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.22
Maniscalco, B., & Lau, H. (2012). A signal detection theoretic approach for estimating metacognitive sensitivity from confidence ratings. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 422–430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.021
Maniscalco, B., & Lau, H. (2014). Signal detection theory analysis of type 1 and type 2 data: Meta-d’, response-specific meta-d’, and the unequal variance SDT model. In S. M. Fleming & C. D. Frith (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of metacognition. (pp. 25–66). Germany: Springer.
Michel, M. (2022). Confidence in consciousness research. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, e1628. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1628
Mills, C., Raffaelli, Q., Irving, Z. C., Stan, D., & Christoff, K. (2018). Is an off-task mind a freely-moving mind? Examining the relationship between different dimensions of thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 58, 20–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.10.003
Mooneyham, B. W., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). The costs and benefits of mind-wandering: A review. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 67(1), 11–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031569
Moreira, C. M., Rollwage, M., Kaduk, K., Wilke, M., & Kagan, I. (2018). Post-decision wagering after perceptual judgments reveals bi-directional certainty readouts. Cognition, 176, 40–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.026
Murray, S., & Krasich, K. (2020). Can the mind wander intentionally? Mind & Language. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12332
Murray, S., Krasich, K., Schooler, J. W., & Seli, P. (2020). What’s in a task? Complications in the study of the task-unrelated-thought variety of mind wandering. Perspectives on Psychological Science : A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 1745691619897966. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619897966
O’Neill, K., Smith, A. P., Smilek, D., & Seli, P. (2021). Dissociating the freely-moving thought dimension of mind-wandering from the intentionality and task-unrelated thought dimensions. Psychological Research, 85(7), 2599–2609. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01419-9
Peirce, J., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M., Höchenberger, R., Sogo, H., Kastman, E., & Lindeløv, J. K. (2019). PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods, 51(1), 195–203. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y
Persaud, N., McLeod, P., & Cowey, A. (2007). Post-decision wagering objectively measures awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 10(2), 257–261. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1840
Peters, M. A. K., & Lau, H. (2015). Human observers have optimal introspective access to perceptual processes even for visually masked stimuli. ELife, 4, e09651. https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.09651
Phillips, I. (2020). Blindsight is qualitatively degraded conscious vision. Psychological Review. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000254
Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). `Oops!’: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), 747–758. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00015-8
Robison, M. K., Miller, A. L., & Unsworth, N. (2019). Examining the effects of probe frequency, response options, and framing within the thought-probe method. Behavior Research Methods, 51(1), 398–408. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01212-6
Samaha, J., Switzky, M., & Postle, B. R. (2019). Confidence boosts serial dependence in orientation estimation. Journal of Vision, 19(4), 25. https://doi.org/10.1167/19.4.25
Schubert, A.-L., Frischkorn, G. T., & Rummel, J. (2020). The validity of the online thought-probing procedure of mind wandering is not threatened by variations of probe rate and probe framing. Psychological Research, 84(7), 1846–1856. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01194-2
Seli, P., Jonker, T. R., Cheyne, J. A., Cortes, K., & Smilek, D. (2015). Can research participants comment authoritatively on the validity of their self-reports of mind wandering and task engagement? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(3), 703–709. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000029
Seli, P., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2016). On the necessity of distinguishing between unintentional and intentional mind wandering. Psychological Science, 27(5), 685–691. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616634068
Seli, P., Risko, E. F., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(8), 605–617. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.010
Seli, P., Wammes, J. D., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2016). On the relation between motivation and retention in educational contexts: The role of intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1280–1287. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0979-0
Smallwood, J., Beach, E., Schooler, J. W., & Handy, T. C. (2008). Going AWOL in the brain: mind wandering reduces cortical analysis of external events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(3), 458–469. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20037
Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2007). The lights are on but no one’s home: Meta-awareness and the decoupling of attention when the mind wanders. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 527–533. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03194102
Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 487–518. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331
Smith, A. P., Brosowsky, N., Murray, S., Daniel, R., Meier, M. E., & Seli, P. (2022). Fixation, flexibility, and creativity: The dynamics of mind wandering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 48(7), 689–710. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001012
Sporer, S. L., Penrod, S., Read, D., & Cutler, B. (1995). Choosing, confidence, and accuracy: A meta-analysis of the confidence–accuracy relation in eyewitness identification studies. Psychological Bulletin, 118(3), 315–327. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.118.3.315
Stanislaw, H., & Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31(1), 137–149. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03207704
Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1096–1101. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1500
Unsworth, N., & Robison, M. K. (2016). The influence of lapses of attention on working memory capacity. Memory & Cognition, 44(2), 188–196. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0560-0
Vinski, M. T., & Watter, S. (2012). Priming honesty reduces subjective bias in self-report measures of mind wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 451–455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.11.001
Weinstein, Y. (2018). Mind-wandering, how do I measure thee with probes? Let me count the ways. Behavior Research Methods, 50(2), 642–661. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0891-9
-
dc.identifier.urihttp://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/83172-
dc.description.abstract  思維探針法是心神漫遊研究中一項重要的研究工具。其施行方式為:在受試者進行作業時,隨機打斷受試者,並詢問受試者當下的思緒狀態為何。透過思維探針法,研究者可以探討心神漫遊前後的行為特徵或認知狀態,或是探討心神漫遊的頻率與其他認知功能之間的關聯性。然而,不是每次給予受試者思維探針時,受試者皆能精準提取自身的思緒狀態。當受試者對思緒狀態的後設覺察低下時,或是當思維探針所問之問題過於困難時,受試者將仰賴自身思緒狀態以外的線索進行回答。我們認為,受試者會有「利用對自身作業表現的主觀評估來回答思維探針」的傾向。若此傾向存在,那麼思維探針法捕捉到的可能就不是受試者的思緒狀態,而是受試者的後設認知偏誤情況。於本研究中,我們利用第二型訊號偵測理論模型量化受試者的後設認知偏誤情況,並探討當受試者覺得自己的作業表現好或壞時,是否有不同的機率回答思維探針中的心神漫遊選項。本研究的三個實驗給予不同思維探針問題,並於實驗中發現:當詢問受試者心神漫遊狀態的意圖性時,受試者會有傾向利用作業表現的主觀評估,回答其心神漫遊狀態的啟動方式,且當受試者的後設覺察越低落時,此傾向則會越強。據此,我們認為後設認知偏誤會威脅思維探針法的建構效度,在思維探針的問題過難,或受試者對自身思緒的後設覺察低下時尤為嚴重。zh_TW
dc.description.abstractThought-probing method is one of the most prominent tools used in mind-wandering studies. This procedure randomly probes participants to halt their ongoing task and report their thoughts at the moment. Researchers can use the thought-probing method to investigate behavioral characteristics or cognitive states before or after mind-wandering episodes, as well as the relationship between mind-wandering episodes and other cognitive functions. However, participants may not accurately retrieve their thoughts every time they receive the probe. When participants' meta-awareness is low or the probe's questions are too difficult, other contextual cues may be used to answer the probe. We hypothesized that participants may respond to the probe according to the subjective evaluations of their own task performance. According to this hypothesis, what is caught by the thought probe is participants' metacognitive bias instead of their genuine mind-wandering episodes. In the current study, in order to study whether different self-evaluations of task performance are accompanied by different degrees of self-reported mind-wandering, we exploit the type 2 signal detection theory model to quantify participants' extent of metacognitive bias. In the three experiments of the current study, we manipulated the questions that were asked in the thought-probing methods. The result of the experiments indicate that when asked about the intention of their mind-wandering episodes, the participants were inclined to base their reports on their self-evaluations of task performance. Moreover, the participants' lower meta-awareness strengthens this tendency. These findings suggest that metacognitive bias threatens the constructive validity of thought-probing methods, especially when demanding probe questions are presented and when participants have insufficient meta-awareness.en
dc.description.provenanceSubmitted by admin ntu (admin@lib.ntu.edu.tw) on 2023-01-10T17:07:20Z
No. of bitstreams: 0
en
dc.description.provenanceMade available in DSpace on 2023-01-10T17:07:20Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 0en
dc.description.tableofcontents論文口試委員審定書 i
中文摘要 ii
英文摘要 iii
第一章 緒論 1
第一節 心神漫遊的定義 1
第二節 思維探針的重要性與多樣性 1
第三節 思維探針法的潛在缺陷 2
第四節 後設覺察對思維探針法建構效度的影響 4
第五節 探針的問題難度對思維探針法建構效度的影響 6
第六節 研究假設和方法概述 8
第二章 實驗一 10
第一節 受試者 10
第二節 實驗設置、材料與流程 10
第三節 資料分析方法 13
第四節 結果與討論 21
第三章 實驗二 25
第一節 受試者 25
第二節 實驗設置、材料與流程 25
第三節 資料分析方法 26
第四節 結果與討論 26
第四章 實驗三 30
第一節 受試者 30
第二節 實驗設置、材料與流程 30
第三節 資料分析方法 31
第四節 結果與討論 31
第五章 綜合討論 35
第一節 研究結果整理 35
第二節 實驗結果的其他解 37
第三節 後設認知偏誤對需主觀報告的實驗典範之效度威脅 40
參考文獻 41
圖 49
-
dc.language.isozh_TW-
dc.title後設認知偏誤威脅心神漫遊研究中思維探針法之建構效度zh_TW
dc.titleMetacognitive Bias Threatens the Constructive Validity of Thought-probing Methods in Mind-wandering Studiesen
dc.title.alternativeMetacognitive Bias Threatens the Constructive Validity of Thought-probing Methods in Mind-wandering Studies-
dc.typeThesis-
dc.date.schoolyear111-1-
dc.description.degree碩士-
dc.contributor.oralexamcommittee黃從仁;徐慈妤zh_TW
dc.contributor.oralexamcommitteeTsung-Ren Huang;Tzu-Yu Hsuen
dc.subject.keyword心神漫遊,後設認知,思維探針,建構效度,認知模型,zh_TW
dc.subject.keywordmind-wandering,metacognition,thought probe,constructive validity,cognitive model,en
dc.relation.page59-
dc.identifier.doi10.6342/NTU202210201-
dc.rights.note同意授權(全球公開)-
dc.date.accepted2023-01-03-
dc.contributor.author-college理學院-
dc.contributor.author-dept心理學系-
顯示於系所單位:心理學系

文件中的檔案:
檔案 大小格式 
U0001-0303221230337133.pdf4.03 MBAdobe PDF檢視/開啟
顯示文件簡單紀錄


系統中的文件,除了特別指名其著作權條款之外,均受到著作權保護,並且保留所有的權利。

社群連結
聯絡資訊
10617臺北市大安區羅斯福路四段1號
No.1 Sec.4, Roosevelt Rd., Taipei, Taiwan, R.O.C. 106
Tel: (02)33662353
Email: ntuetds@ntu.edu.tw
意見箱
相關連結
館藏目錄
國內圖書館整合查詢 MetaCat
臺大學術典藏 NTU Scholars
臺大圖書館數位典藏館
本站聲明
© NTU Library All Rights Reserved