請用此 Handle URI 來引用此文件:
http://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/62304
完整後設資料紀錄
DC 欄位 | 值 | 語言 |
---|---|---|
dc.contributor.advisor | 蔡振家 | |
dc.contributor.author | Chung-Ping Chen | en |
dc.contributor.author | 陳宗平 | zh_TW |
dc.date.accessioned | 2021-06-16T13:39:52Z | - |
dc.date.available | 2015-07-19 | |
dc.date.copyright | 2013-07-19 | |
dc.date.issued | 2013 | |
dc.date.submitted | 2013-07-15 | |
dc.identifier.citation | 徐頌仁,《音樂演奏的實務探討》,台北:全音樂譜,1992。
張春興,《教育心理學》,頁36,台北:東華,2008。 張愛玲,〈談音樂〉,《張愛玲文集》第四卷,安徽文藝出版社,1992。 傑夫.霍金斯、珊卓.布萊克斯利 (Jeff Hawkins and Sandra Blakeslee),《創智慧:理解人腦運作,打造智慧機器》,洪蘭譯,台北:遠流,2006。 楊為茜、蔡振家,〈西樂敘說紅色神話─音樂基模轉換在京劇樣板戲中的功能與實踐〉,《戲劇學刊》13期 (2011),頁 131-157。 蔡振家、陳容姍,〈渴望.解決.酬賞系統:聽眾對於終止式的情緒反應〉,《藝術學報》90期 (2012),頁325-345。 蘇珊.麥克拉蕊 (Susan McClary),《陰性終止》(Feminine Endings),張馨濤譯,台北:商周,2003。 Agawu, Victor Kofi. Playing with Signs: A Semiotic Interpretation of Classic Music, Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1991. Balkwill, L. L., and Thompson, W. F.. “A Cross-cultural Investigation of the Perception of Emotion in Music: Psychophysical and Cultural Uues.” Music Perception 17 (1999): 43-64. Bandt, R., M. Duffy, and D. MacKinnon, eds. Hearing Places: Sound, Place, Time and Culture. Newcastle, England: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, 2007. Benedek, M., and C. Kaernbach. “Physiological Correlates and Emotional Specificity of Human Piloerection.” Biological Psychology 86/3 (2011): 320-329. Benward, Bruce, and Marilyn Nadine Saker. Music in Theory and Practice, eighth edition, vol. 2. Boston: McGraw-Hill, 2009. Berry, Wallace. Form in Music. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 1966. Bharucha, J. J.. “Music Cognition and Perceptual Facilitation: A Connectionist Framework.” Music Perception 5/1 (1987): 1-30. Bigand, E., R. Parncutt, and F. Lerdahl. “Perception of Musical Tension in Short Chord Sequences: The Influence of Harmonic Function, Sensory Dissonance, Horizontal Motion, and Musical Training.” Perception & Psychophysics 58 (1996): 125-141. Bigand, E., and R. Parncutt. “Perception of Musical Tension in Long Chord Sequences.” Psychological Research 62 (1999): 237-254. Blood A. J., and R. J. Zatorre. “Intensely Pleasurable Responses to Music Correlate with Activity in Brain Regions Implicated in Reward and Emotion.” Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98/20 (2001): 11818-11823. Boudewyns, P. A.. “A Comparison of the Effects of Stress vs. Relaxation Instruction on the Finger Temperature Response.” Behavior Therapy 7/1 (1976): 54-67. Bugental D. B., and V. L. Cortez. “Physiological Reactivity to Responsive and Unresponsive Children as Moderated by Perceived Control.” Child Development 59/3 (1988): 686-693. Carlsen, J. C.. “Some Factors Which Influence Melodic Expectancy.” Psychomusicology 1/1 (1981): 12-29. Chen, Wen-Mi. “An Analysis of Sonata Form in Clarinet Concertos by Wolfgang Amadeus Mozart, Louis Spohr, and Carl Maria von Weber.” Ph.D. diss., University of Cincinnati, 2012. Cooper, G. W., and L. B. Meyer. The Rhythmic Structure of Music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960. Cook, Nicholas. “The Perception of Large-Scale Tonal Closure.” Music Perception 5 (1987): 197-205. Cuddy L. L., and C. A. Lunny. “Expectancies Generated by Melodic Intervals: Perceptual Judgements of Continuity.” Perception and Psychophysics 57/4 (1995): 451-462. Czemy, Carl, and John Bishop, eds. Course of Musical Composition or Complete and Methodical Treatise of Practical Harmony. London: Robert Cocks, 1854. Damasio, A.. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: G. P. Putnam’s Sons, 1994. Farbood, M. M.. “A Quantitative, Parametric Model of Musical Tension.” Ph.D. diss., Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2001. Fredrickson, W. E.. “A Comparison of Perceived Musical Tension and Aesthetic Response.” Psychology of Music 23 (1995): 81-87. Goldman, Richard Franko. “Varèse: Ionisation; Density 21.5; Intégrales; Octandre; Hyperprism; Poème Electronique. Instrumentalists, cond. Robert Craft. Columbia MS 6146 (stereo)” (in Reviews of Records). Musical Quarterly 47/1 (1961): 133-134. Gomez, P., and B. Danuser. “Affective and Physiological Responses to Environmental Noises and Music.” International Journal of Psychophysiology 53 (2004): 91-103. Gomez, P., and B. Danuser. “Relationships Between Musical Structure and Psychophysiological Measures of Emotion.” Emotion 7/2 (2007): 377-387. Granot, Roni Y., and Zohar Eitan, “Musical Tension and the Interaction of Dynamic Auditory Parameters,” Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal 28/3 (February 2011): 219-246. Granot, Roni Y., and Nori Jacoby, “Musically Puzzling I: Sensitivity to Overall Structure in the Sonata Form?” Musicae Scientiae 15/3 (2011): 365-386. Gross, J. J., and R. W. Levenson, “Hiding Feelings: The Acute Effects of Inhibiting Negative and Positive Emotion.” Journal of Abnormal Psychology 106/1 (1997): 95-103. Guhn, M., A. Hamm, and M. Zentner. “Physiological and Musico-acoustic Correlates of the Chill Response.” Music Perception 24 (2007): 473-483. Hirota A., and H. Hirai. “Effects of Stimulus-Oriented or Response-Oriented Training on Psychophysiological Responses and the Propositional Structure of Imagery.” Japanese Psychological Research 28/4 (1986): 186-195. Hirota A., and H. Hirai. “Effects of Relaxation-Response-Oriented or Arousal-Response-Oriented Training on Psychophysiological Responses during Fear Imagery.” Japanese Psychological Research 32/1 (1990): 26-35. Huron, David. Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2006. Insko, Brent E.. “Measuring Presence: Subjective, Behavioral and Physiological Methods.” In Being There: Concepts, Effects and Measurements of User Presence in Synthetic Environments, edited by Giuseppe Riva, Fabrizio Davide, and Wijnand Ijsselsteijn, Amsterdam: IOS Press, 2003. Iwanaga, M., A. Kobayashi, and C. Kawasaki. “Heart rate Variability with Repetitive Exposure to Music.” Biol Psychol 70/1 (2005): 61-66. Jones, M. R.. “Dynamic Pattern Structure in Music: Recent Theory and Research.” Perception and Psychophysics 41/6 (1987): 621-634. Jones M. R., and M. G. Boltz. “Dynamic Attending and Responses to Time.” Psychological Review 96/3(1989): 459-491. Juslin P., and D. Västfjall. “Emotional Responses to Music: The Need to Consider Underlying Mechanisms.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (2008): 5559-5575. Justus, T. C., and J. J. Bharucha. “Modularity in Musical Processing: The Automaticity of Harmonic Priming.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 27 (2001): 1000-1011. Kamenetsky, Stuart B., David S. Hill, and Sandra E. Trehub. “Effect of Tempo and Dynamics on the Perception of Emotion in Music.” Psychology of Music 25 (1997): 149-160. Khalfa, S., P. Isabelle, B. Jean-Pierre, and R. Manon. “Event-related Skin Conductance Responses to Musical Emotions in Humans.” Neuroscience Letters 328/2 (2002): 145-149. Khalfa, S., M. Roy , P. Rainville, S. Dalla Bella, and I. Peretz. “Role of Tempo Entrainment in Psychophysiological Differentiation of Happy and Sad Music?” Int J Psychophysiol 68/1 (2008): 17-26. Kibler V. E., and M. S. Rider. “Effects of Progressive Muscle-Relaxation and Music on Stress as Measured by Finger Temperature Response.” Journal of Clinical Psychology 39/2 (1983): 213-215. Koch, H.C.. Versuch einer Anleitung zur Composition, Vol. 2. Leipzig: Adam F. Bohme, 1787. Krumhansl, C. L.. “Effects of Musical Context on Similarity and Expectancy.” Systematische Musikwissenschaft, 3/2 (1995): 211-250. Krumhansl, C. L.. “A Perceptual Analysis of Mozart’s Piano Sonata, K. 282: Segmentation, Tension and Musical Ideas.” Music Perception 13 (1996): 401-432. Krumhansl, C. L.. “An Exploratory Study of Musical Emotions and Psychophysiology.” Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 51 (1997): 336-353. Krumhansl, C. L.. “Music: A link between Cognition and Emotion.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 11 (2002): 45-50. Krumhansl, C. L., and D. L. Schenck. “Can Dance Reflect the Structural and Expressive Qualities of Music? A Perceptual Experiment on Balanchine’s Choreography of Mozart’s Divertimento No. 15.” Musicae Scientiae 1 (Spring, 1997): 63-85. Kunzmann U., and D. Gruhn. “Age Differences in Emotional Reactivity: The Sample Case of Sadness.” Psychology and Aging 20/1 (2005): 47-59. Laczika, Klaus, Oliver P. Graber, Gerhard Tucek, Alfred Lohninger, Nikolaus Fliri, Gertraud Berka-Schmid, Eva K. Masel, and Christoph C. Zielinski. “'Il flauto Magico' Still Works: Mozart's Secret of Ventilation.” Multidisciplinary Respiratory Medicine 8/1 (2013): 23-30. Large E. W., and M. R. Jones. “The Dynamics of Attending: How We Track Time-varying Events.” Psychological Review 106 (1999): 119-159. Larson, S.. “The Problem of Prolongation in Tonal Music: Terminology, Perception, and Expressive Meaning.” Journal of Music Theory 41 (1997): 101-136. Larson, S.. “Musical Forces and Melodic Expectations: Comparing Computer Models with Experimental Results.” Music Perception 21 (2004): 457-498. Lee, K. C., Y. H. Chao, J. J. Yiin, P. Y. Chiang, and Y. F. Chao. “Effectiveness of Different Music-playing Devices for Reducing Preoperative Anxiety: A Clinical Control Study.” Int J Nurs Stud 48/10 (2011): 1180-1187. Lerdahl, Fred, and C. L. Krumhansl. “Modeling Tonal Tension.” Music Perception 24/4 (2007): 329-366. Lester, J.. The Rhythms of Tonal Music. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press, 1986. Lewis, P. A.. “Musical Minds.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6/9 (2002): 364-366. Lundqvist, L. O., F. Carlsson, P. Hilmersson, and P. N. Juslin. “Emotional Responses to Music: Experience, Expression, and Physiology.” Psychology of Music 37/1 (2009): 61-90. Madsen, C. K., and W. E. Fredrickson. “The Experience of Musical Tension: A Replication of Nielsen’s Research Using the Continuous Response Digital Interface.” Journal of Music Therapy 30 (1993): 46-63. Margulis, Elizabeth Hellmuth. “A Model of Melodic Expectation,” Music Perception 22/4 (2005): 663–714. Masson, M. E.. “Comprehension of Rapidly Presented Sentences: The Mind Is Quicker than the Eye.” Journal of Memory and Language 25 (1986): 588-604. Masterson, Sarah E.. “Approaches to Sonata Form in Mid-Twentieth-Century American Piano Sonatas.” Ph.D. diss., University of Connecticut, 2011. Matsumoto, K., W. Suzuki, and K. Tanaka. “Nueronal Correlates of Goal-based Motor Selection in the Prefrontal Cortex.” Science 301 (2003): 229-232. McAdams, S., B. W. Vines, S. Vieillard, B. K. Smith, and R. Reynolds. “Influences of Large-scale form on Continuous Ratings in Response to a Contemporary Piece in a Live Concert Setting.” Music Perception 22 (2004): 297-350. McClary, Susan. Feminine Endings: Music, Gender, and Sexuality, 2nd. ed.. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 2002. Meyer, Leonard B.. Emotion and Meaning in Music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1956. Momigny, Jérôme-Joseph de. Cours complet d'harmonie et de composition. Paris: de Momigny et Bailleul, 1803. Narmour, E.. The Analysis and Cognition of Basic Melodic Structures: The Implication-realisation Model. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1990. Narmour, E.. The Analysis and Cognition of Melodic Complexity: The Implication-realisation Model. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1992. Nater, U. M., E. Abbruzzese, M. Krebs, and U. Ehlert. “Sex differences in Emotional and Psychophysiological Responses to Musical Stimuli.” Int J Psychophysiol 62/2 (2006): 300-308. Newman, William S.. “The Climax of Music.” Music Review 13 (1952): 283-293. North, A., and D. Hargreaves. “Subjective Complexity, Familiarity, and Liking for Popular Music.” Psychomusicology 14 (1995): 77-93. Patel, A. D.. “Language, Music, Syntax and the Brain.” Nature Neuroscience 6 (2003): 674-681. Pearce, M. T., and Wiggins, G. A.. “Auditory Expectation: The Information Dynamics of Music Perception and Cognition.” TopiCS in Cognitive Science 4 (2012): 625-652. Peretz, I., and M. Coltheart. “Modularity of Music Processing.” Nature Neuroscience 6 (2003): 688-691. Posner, M. I., and C. R. R. Snyder, “Attention and cognitive control.” In Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium, edited by R. L. Solso, 55-85. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers, 1975. Pressnitzer, D., S. Mcadams, S. Winsberg, and J. Fineberg, “Perception of Music Tension for Nontonal Orchestral Timbres and Its Relation to Psychoacoustic Roughness.” Perception and Psychophysics 62 (2000): 66-80. Rakic, Pasko. “Evolution of the Neocortex: A Perspective from Developmental Biology.” Nat Rev Neurosci 10 (2009): 724-735. Randel, D. M.. The Harvard Concise Dictionary of Music and Musicians. Cambridge, Mass: Belknap Press, 1999. Reicha, Anton. Traitd de haute composition musicale, Vol. 2. Paris: Chez A. Farrence et chez l'auteur, 1824-1826. Richmond, B. J., Z. Liu, and M. Shidara. “Predicting Future Rewards.” Science 301 (2003): 179-180. Rickard, N.. “Intense Emotional Responses to Music: A Test of the Physiological Arousal Hypotheses.” Psychology of Music 32 (2004): 371-388. Rosen, Charles. The Classical Style: Haydn, Mozart, Beethoven. New York: The Viking Press, 1971. Rosen, Charles. Sonata Forms, rev. ed. New York: WW Norton, 1988. Russell, J. A.. “Affective Space Is Bipolar.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37 (1979): 345-356. Rosner , B. S., and Leonard B. Meyer. “The Perceptual Roles of Melodic Process, Contour and Form.” Music Perception 4 (1986): 1-39. Schellenberg, E. G.. “Expectancy in Melody: Tests of the Implication-realisation Model.” Cognition 58/1 (1996): 75-125. Schmuckler, M. A. “Expectation in Music: Investigation of Melodic and Harmonic Processes.” Music Perception 7/2 (1989): 109-150. Schmuckler, M. A.. “The Performance of Global Expectations,” Psychomusicology 9/2 (1990): 122-147 Schmuckler, M. A.. “Expectancy Effects in Memory for Melodies.” Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 51/4 (1997): 292-305. Schubert, E., P. N. Juslin, and J. A. Sloboda, eds. “Continuous Measurement of Self-report Emotional Response to Music.” In Music and emotion: Theory and research, 393-414. New York: Oxford University Press. Shamgar, Beth. “ Rhythmic Interplay in the Retransitions of Haydn's Piano Sonatas.” The Journal of Musicology 3/1 (Winter, 1984): 55-68. Simon, Carol R., and Joachim F. Wohlwill. “An Experimental Study of the Role of Expectation and Variation in Music.” Journal of Research in Music Education 16/3 (Autumn, 1968): 227-238. Sinha R., and O. A. Parsons. “Multivariate Response Patterning of Fear and Anger.” Cognition and Emotion 10/2 (1996): 173-198. Spitzer, Michael. “The Retransition as Sign: Listener-Orientated Approaches to Tonal Closure in Haydn's Sonata-Form Movements.” Journal of the Royal Musical Association 121/1 (1996): 11-45. Steinbeis, N., S. Koelsch, and J. A. Sloboda. “The Role of Harmonic Expectancy Violations in Musical Emotions: Evidence from Subjective, Physiological and Neural Responses.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18/8 (2006): 1380-1393. Stemmler, G.. “The Autonomic Differentiation of Emotions Revisited: Convergent and Discriminant Validation.” Psychophysiology 26/6 (1989): 617-632. Storr, A. The Dynamics of Creation. New York: Atheneum, 1972. Thompson, W. F., L. L. Cuddy, and C. Plaus. “Expectancies Generated by Melodic Intervals: Evaluation of Principles of Melodic Implication in a Melody-completion Task.” Perception and Psychophysics 59/7 (1997): 1069-1076. Thyer, B. A., J. D.Papsdorf, R. Davis, and S. Vallecorsa. “Autonomic Correlates of the Subjective Anxiety Scale.” Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 15/1 (1984): 3-7. Tillmann, B. , E. Bigand, and M. Pineau. “Effects of Global and Local Contexts on Harmonic Expectancy.” Music Perception 16 (1998): 99-118. Tillmann, B., J. Bharucha, and E. Bigand, “Implicit Learning of Tonality: A Self-organizing Approach.” Psychol Rev 107 (2000): 885-913. Tucker, G. M., and Nicholas Temperley. “Sonata Form.” In The Oxford Companion to Music, edited by Alison Latham. Oxford Music Online, http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/opr/t114/e6317 (accessed September 26, 2012) . Vines, Bradley W., Regina L. Nuzzo, and Daniel J. Levitin. “ Analyzing Temporal Dynamics in Music.” Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal 23/2 (December 2005): 137-152. Webster, James. “Sonata Form.” Grove Music Online, Oxford Music Online. Oxford University, accessed September 26, 2012, Press,http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/grove/music/26197. Webster, James. “Retransition.” Grove Music Online, Oxford Music Online. Oxford University Press, accessed February 22, 2013, http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/grove/music/23261. White, John David. The Analysis of Music. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1976. Wingfield, Paul. “Beyond ‘Norms and Deformations’: Towards a Theory of Sonata Form as Reception History,” Music Analysis 27/1 (2008): 137-177. YL, Ko, and Lin PC. “The Effect of Using a Relaxation Tape on Pulse, Respiration, Blood Pressure and Anxiety Levels of Surgical Patients.” J Clin Nurs 21 (2012): 689-697. Zavortink, Matt B.. “The Influence and Application of Eastern Philosophy in the Western Musical Tradition.” Summer Research (2011): 123-139. | |
dc.identifier.uri | http://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/62304 | - |
dc.description.abstract | 再過渡樂段 (retransition) 是奏鳴曲式中發展部末端用以返回主調,並迎向再現的一個過程。雖然已有不少理論家認同再過渡樂段於音樂聆聽中所具有的情感地位,其本質與內涵仍少有人探索。由於再過渡樂段的長度短小,以及本質上的過渡性,因而未獲得重視。在音樂分析上,再過渡樂段亦不被視為一個完整的結構單位,僅是隸屬於發展部的一小部分。
然而,再過渡樂段的重要性卻可從三個層面中突顯出來。第一,再過渡樂段被賦予了轉化發展與指示再現的功能。第二,再過渡樂段經常是有效之聽覺與認知上的標的。第三,再過渡樂段為奏鳴曲式中最重要的劇碼——與樂曲主調素材一致出現的張力解決——設置了表現的舞台。 本研究嘗試從預期的角度出發,探討主題再現之際的張力-解決模式。首先,藉由觀察再過渡樂段導入再現部的方式,將奏鳴曲式分為「連體型」與「中止型」兩大類;其次,提出幾項與音樂預期相關之假設,並統計再過渡樂段所具有之音樂特徵,從而探究其相似性與同質性;最後以聆聽行為實驗檢視聽者聆聽再過渡樂段至再現部的生理激發模式。 實驗結果顯示,再過渡樂段行進至再現部期間經常伴隨了膚電值的上升與呼吸深度的變化。膚電反應說明了再過渡樂段推移至再現部是一個營建張力並使之解決的過程;而進入再現部之後所導致的呼吸深度增加,則可以表示為再現符應了聽者的預期,繼而於其心中激發甜美的愉悅感受。 | zh_TW |
dc.description.abstract | “Retransition” is a transition in the end of the development of the Sonata form, used to return to the tonic, leading to the recapitulation. Although many theorists have recognized the affective role of the retransition in music listening, the essence of the retransition is still not explored. Because of its shortness in length and transitional nature, the retransition has gotten no attention for a long time. It was also not considered as a complete section, but a small unit being attached to the development.
However, the importance of retransition would be obvious if we consider three aspects: first, the retransition works as a closure of development and a sign of recapitulation; second, it is usually a significant event in listening and cognition; third, it sets the stage for what is one of the central events in the Sonata form - the release of tension that coincides with the reappearance of the opening material in the tonic. This study aims at examining the tension-relaxation patterns at the retransition to the recapitulation with the perspective of expectation. First, the study classifies the Sonata form into two main types, “continuity” and “discontinuity”; then addresses some expectation-related hypotheses, and analyzes the similarity of the retransition by calculating its musical characters; finally, surveys the physiological states of listeners while listening to the retransition. The result of physiological measurement shows that there are usually increases of skin conductance and respiration depth while listening to the retransition. The response of skin conductance display that the retransition is a progress of constructing musical tension; while the increase of respiration depth can be interpreted as satisfaction of listeners for the recapitulations meet their expectations. | en |
dc.description.provenance | Made available in DSpace on 2021-06-16T13:39:52Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1 ntu-102-R99144008-1.pdf: 8862063 bytes, checksum: d8beff0c673729843358c3624eb3d246 (MD5) Previous issue date: 2013 | en |
dc.description.tableofcontents | 第一章 緒論 1
第一節 研究動機 1 第二節 研究範疇及目的 3 第三節 文獻回顧 7 第四節 研究方法與限制 13 第二章 再過渡樂段及其張力形塑/解決 19 第一節 再過渡樂段與區域性聆聽 19 第二節 音樂的張力 21 第三節 預期與再過渡樂段的張力 25 第三章 再過渡樂段之特徵分析與統計 30 第一節 再過渡樂段導入再現部之種類 30 第二節 音樂特徵統計 41 第三節 小結 44 第四章 再過渡樂段的聆聽行為研究 46 第一節 實驗設計 46 第二節 生理測量結果與六段刺激材料之關聯分析 52 第三節 討論 67 第四節 小結 72 第五章 結論 74 參考文獻 79 | |
dc.language.iso | zh-TW | |
dc.title | 奏鳴曲式主題再現之際的張力—解決模式:音樂分析與聽眾生理測量之研究 | zh_TW |
dc.title | Tension-relaxation Patterns at the Retransition to the Recapitulation in the Sonata Form: A Study Combining Music Analysis with Listeners’ Physiological Measures | en |
dc.type | Thesis | |
dc.date.schoolyear | 101-2 | |
dc.description.degree | 碩士 | |
dc.contributor.oralexamcommittee | 蘇郁惠,陳一平 | |
dc.subject.keyword | 再過渡樂段,音樂張力,預期, | zh_TW |
dc.subject.keyword | retransition,musical tension,expectation, | en |
dc.relation.page | 86 | |
dc.rights.note | 有償授權 | |
dc.date.accepted | 2013-07-15 | |
dc.contributor.author-college | 文學院 | zh_TW |
dc.contributor.author-dept | 音樂學研究所 | zh_TW |
顯示於系所單位: | 音樂學研究所 |
文件中的檔案:
檔案 | 大小 | 格式 | |
---|---|---|---|
ntu-102-1.pdf 目前未授權公開取用 | 8.65 MB | Adobe PDF |
系統中的文件,除了特別指名其著作權條款之外,均受到著作權保護,並且保留所有的權利。