Skip navigation

DSpace

機構典藏 DSpace 系統致力於保存各式數位資料(如:文字、圖片、PDF)並使其易於取用。

點此認識 DSpace
DSpace logo
English
中文
  • 瀏覽論文
    • 校院系所
    • 出版年
    • 作者
    • 標題
    • 關鍵字
  • 搜尋 TDR
  • 授權 Q&A
    • 我的頁面
    • 接受 E-mail 通知
    • 編輯個人資料
  1. NTU Theses and Dissertations Repository
  2. 理學院
  3. 心理學系
請用此 Handle URI 來引用此文件: http://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/54809
完整後設資料紀錄
DC 欄位值語言
dc.contributor.advisor張素凰(Sue-Hwang Chang)
dc.contributor.authorHong-Yi Yehen
dc.contributor.author葉弘毅zh_TW
dc.date.accessioned2021-06-16T03:38:57Z-
dc.date.available2018-03-16
dc.date.copyright2015-03-16
dc.date.issued2015
dc.date.submitted2015-02-25
dc.identifier.citation中央研究院語言研究所(2009)。「中文詞頻資料庫」。台北:中央研究院。
朱靜怡(2009)。「不同程度懼蟑者之注意力偏誤:時間序列之角色」。台北:國立台灣大學心理學研究所碩士論文。
姚開屏(2001)。「世界衛生組織生活量表」(台灣簡版)。台灣版世界衛生組織生活品質問卷之發展及使用手冊,世界衛生組織生活品質問卷台灣版問卷發展小組。
教育部國語推行委員會(2007)。「重編國語辭典修訂本相似詞資料庫」。台北:教育部。
張素凰(1987)。中文版莫斯利強迫量表的修訂。未發表之手稿。
湯華盛(2005)。「薛西佛斯也瘋狂:強迫症的認識與治療」。台北:張老師。
黃政昌(2003)。「強迫症門診患者的臨床特徵與聯合治療模式效果之分析研究」。台北:國立台灣師範大學教育心理與輔導研究所博士論文。
黃瑜珮(2008)。「高強迫傾向者之偏誤性注意特性探討」。台北:國立台灣大學 心理學研究所碩士論文。
黃瑜珮、張素凰(2008)。高強迫傾向者之偏向性注意特性探討。「第47屆台灣心理學年會」。台灣師範大學,台北市。
劉英茂、吳瑞屯(1990)。「常用敘述單位的出現次數」。香港:中文大學出版社。
鄧閔鴻(2010)。「廣泛性焦慮疾患的心理病理:病態憂慮、注意偏誤、與憂慮後設認知」。台北:國立台灣大學心理學研究所博士論文。
鄧閔鴻、張素凰(2014)。注意偏誤操弄的潛在機制在類廣泛性焦慮疾患上的探討:投注注意與注意轉移困難的角色。「中華心理學刊」,59,355-358。
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.,text vision : DSM-IV).Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.,text revision : DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.text vision : DSM-5).Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Amir, N., Elias, J., Klumpp, H., & Przeworski, A. (2003). Attentional bias to threat in social phobia: Facilitated processing of threat or difficulty disengaging attention from threat? Behaviour Research and Therapy,41(11),1325-1335.
Amir, N., Beard, C., Burns, M., & Bomyea, J. (2009). Attention modification program in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 28-33.
Antony, M. M., Downie, F., & Swinson, R. P. (1998).Diagnostic issues and epidemiology in obsessive-compulsive disorder. In Swinson, R. P., Antony, M.M., Rachman, S., & Richter, M. A (Eds.), Obsessive-compulsive disorders: Theory, research and treatment (pp. 3-32). New York:Guilford.
Antony, M. M., Purden, C. L., Huta V., & Swinson, R. P. (1998). Dimensions of perfectionism across the anxiety disorders. Behaviors Research and Therapy, 36,1143-1154.
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious Individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin,133(1), 1-24.
Barlow, D. H. (1986). Generalized anxiety and generalized anxiety disorder: Description and reconceptualization. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 143(1),40-44.
Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (1985). Anxiety disorders and phobias : A cognitive perspective, New York: Basic Books.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A., Shaw, B., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therpy of depression.New York: Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
Becker, E. S., Rinck, M., Margraf, J., & Roth, W. T. (2001). The emotional Stroop effect in anxiety disorders: General emotionality or disorder specificity? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 15(3), 147-159.
Becker, E. S., Goodwin, R., Holting, C., Hoyer, J., & Margraf, J. (2003). Content of worry in the community: What do people with generalized anxiety disorder or other disorders worry about? Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,191(10), 688-691.
Bond, N. W., & Siddle, D. A. T. (1996). Current controversies in the anxiety disorders. New York: Guilford Press.
Borkovec, T. D., Robinson, E., Pruzinsky, T., & DePree, J. A. (1983). Preliminary exploration of worry: Some characteristics and processes.Behaviour Research & Therapy, 21(1), 9-16.
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory.American Psychologist, 36(2), 129-248.
Bower, G. H. (1987). Commentary on mood and memory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25, 443-455.
Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Millar, N., & White, J. (1995). Selective processing of negative information: Effects of clinical anxiety, concurrent depression, and awareness. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 532-532.
Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Flla, S. J., & Hailton, L. R. (1998). Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: Manipulation of stimulus duration.Cognition and Emotion,12(6), 737-753.
Brown, K., Ryan, R., & Creswell, J. (2007). Mindfulness: theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18(4), 211.
Burgess, I. S., Jones, L. M., Robertson, S. A., Radcliffe, W. N., & Emerson, E.(1981). The degree of control exerted by phobic and non-phobic verbal stimulus over the recognition behaviour of phobic and non-phobic subjects. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 19, 233-243.
Calvo, M. G., & Avero, P. (2005). Time course of attentional bias to emotional scenesin anxiety: Gaze direction and duration. Cognition & Emotion, 19(3), 433-451
Cartwright-Hatton, S., & Wells, A. (1997). Beliefs about worry and intrusions: The meta-cognitions questionnaire and its correlates. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 11(3), 279–296.
Chu, C. I., & Chang, S. H. (2009). Attentional bias during visual threat processing in different degree of cockroach fear individuals: A time course analysis. Paper presented at the European Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Therapy (EABCT 39th), Dubrovnik, Croatia.
Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 203-216.
Cohen, Y., Lachenmeyer, J. R., & Springer, C. (2003). Anxiety and selective attention in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(11), 1311-1323.
Constans, J. I., Barbee, J. G., Townsend, M. H., & Leffler, H. (2002). The cognitive content of naturally occurring worry episodes. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26(2), 167-177.
Dalgleish, T., Watts , F. N. (1990). Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 589-604.
Dalgleish, T., Taghavi, R., Neshat-Doost, H., Moradi, A., Canterbury, R., & Yule, W. (2003). Patterns of processing bias for emotional information across clinical disorders: A comparison of attention, memory, and prospective cognition in children and adolescents with depression, generalized anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32(1), 10-21.
Dennis, T. A., Chen, C. C., & McCandliss, B. D. (2008). Threat-related attentional biases:An analysis of three attention systems. Depress Anxiety.25(6): E1- E10
Dugas, M. J., Marchand, A., & Ladouceur, R. (2005). Further validation of a cognitive behavioral model of generalized anxiety disorder: diagnostic and symptom specificity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19, 329–343.
Ellis, A. ( 1994 ). Reason and emotion in psychotherapy revised. New York: Kensington.
Ellis, A. ( 1999 ). How to make yourself happy and remarkably less disturbable. Atascadern, CA: Impact.
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(1), 336-353.
Frewen, P. A., Dozois, D. J., Joanisse, M. F., & Neufeld, R. W. (2008). Selective attention to threat versus reward: Meta-analysis and neural-network modeling of the dot-probe task. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(2), 307-337.
Foa, E. B., Feske, U., Murdock, T. B., Kozak, M. J., & McCarthy, P. R. (1991).Processing of threat-related information in rape victims. Journal of AbnormalPsychology, 100(2), 156-162.
Foa, E.B. and McNally, R. J. C. (1986). Sensitivity to feared stimuli in obsessive-compulsives: A dichotic listening analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research,10,477-486.
Fontenelle, L. F., Mendlowicz, M. V., & Versiani, M. (2005). Clinical subtypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder based on the presence of checking and washing compulsions. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 27(3), 201-207.
Grant, D. M., & Beck, J. G. (2006). Attentional biases in social anxiety and dysphoria: does comorbidity make a difference? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20(4), 520-529.
Goodman, W., Price, L., Rasmussen, S., Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R., & Hill, C.(1989). The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale, I: Development, use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 1006-1011.
Hamilton, M (1959) The assessment of anxiety states by rating. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 32, 50 -55.
Hamilton, M (1960) A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. 23, 56-62.
Hazen, R. A., Vasey, M. W., & Schmidt, N. B. (2009). Attentional retraining: A randomized clinical trial for pathological worry. Journal of Psychiatric Research,
43(6), 627-633.
Helm, H. W. J., & Boward, M. D. (2003). Factor structure of the Beck Depression Inventory in a university sample. Psychological Reports, 92(1), 53-61.
Hishinuma, E. S., Miyamoto, R. H., Nishimura, S. T., Nahulu, L. B., Andrade, N. N., Makani, G. K., Yuen, N. Y. C., Johnson, R. C., Kim, S. P., Goebert, D. A., & Guererro, A. P. S. (2000). Psychometric properties of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Asian/Pacific-islander adolescents. Assessment, 7, 17-36.
Hodgson, R., & Rachman, S. (1977). Obsessional-compulsive complaints. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15(5), 389-395.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: H. Holt and Company
Janeck, A. S., Calamari, J. E., Riemann, B. C., & Heffelfinger, S.K. (2003). Too much thinking about thinking ? Metacognitive differences in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17(2), 181–195.
Jenike, M. A. (1990). Predictors of treatment failure, in obsessive compulsive disorders: Theory and management (2nd ed). Chicago: Year Book Medical.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Mindfulness meditation for everyday live. London: Piatkus Books.
Kampman, M., Ger, P. J. K., Marc, J. P. M. V., Gerard, N., & Cees, A. L. H. (2002).The emotional stroop: A comparison of panic disorder patients, obsessive-compulsive patients, and normal controls, in two experiments.Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16(4), 425-441.
Karno, M., Golding, J. M., Sorenson, S. B., Burnam, M. A.(1988). The epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder in five US communities. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 45(12), 1094-1099.
Keo, H. F., & Chang, S. H. (2005). Responsibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder.Paper presented at the TPA 2005 Annual Conference, Taipei.
Khanna, S., & Mukherjee, D. (1992). Checkers and washers: Valid subtype of obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychopathology, 25(1), 283-288.
Koran, L. M., Thienemann, M. L., & Davenport, R. (1996). Quality of life for patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153(6),783-788.
Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(10), 1183-1192.
Koster, E. H. W., Bruno, V., Geert, C., & Stefaan, V. D. (2005). Time-course of attention for threatening pictures in high and low trait anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1087-1098.
Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Attention to threat in anxiety-prone individuals:Mechanisms underlying attentional bias.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30(5), 635-643.
Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., Van Damme, S., & Wiersema, J. R. (2006). Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety:Facilitate dengagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(12), 1757-1771.
Kring, A. M. Davison, G. C., Neale, J. M., & Johnson, S. L. (2007). Abnormal psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc.Lavy, E. H., van Oppen, P., & van den Hout, M. A. (1994). Selective processing ofemotional information in obsessive compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(2), 243-246.
Leckman, J. F., Grice, D. E., Boardman, J., & Zhang, H. (1997). Symptoms of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(7), 911-917.
Lee, Y. D., Chi, C. H., Li, C. Y., Chang, I., & Chen, C. C. (1998). Reliability of HDRS and HARS tele-psychiatric assessment of anxiety and depression. Taiwan Journal of Psychiatry, 12(3), 64-74.
Lensi, P., Cassano, G. B., Correddu, G., Ravagli, S., Kunovac, J. L., & Akiskal, H. S. (1996) . Obsessive-compulsive disorder. Familial-developmental history, symptomatology, comorbidity, and course with special reference to gender-related differences. British Journal of Psychiatry, 169(1), 101-107.
Lim, S. L., & Kim, J. H. (2005). Cognitive processing of emotional information in depression, panic, and somatoform disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(1), 50-61.
Lipp, O. V., & Derakshan, N. (2005). Attentional bias to pictures of fear-relevant animals in a dot probe task. Emotion, 5(3), 365-369.
Luecken, L. J., Tartaro, J., & Appelhans, B. (2004). Strategic coping responses and attentional biases. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 28, 23-37.
MacDonald, B., & Davey, G. C. L. E. (2005). Inflated responsibility and perseverative checking: The effect of negative mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(1), 176-182.
MacDonald, P. A., Antony, M. M., MacLeod, C. M., & Richter, M. A. (1997). Memory and confidence in memory judgments among individuals with obsessive compulsive disorder and non-clinical controls. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
35(6), 497-505.
MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002).Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(1), 107-123.
MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), 15-20.
Marazziti, D., Dell'Osso, L., Di Nasso, E., Pfanner, C., Presta, S., Mungai, F., & Cassano, G. B. (2002). Insight in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A study of an Italian sample. European Psychiatry, 17(7), 407-410.
Martin, M., Williams, R. M., & Clark, D. M. (1991). Does anxiety lead to selective process of threat-related
information. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29(2),147-160.
Mataix-Cols, D., Wooderson, S., Lawrence, N., Brammer, M. J., Speckens, A., & Phillips, M. L. (2004). Distinct neural correlates of washing, checking, and hoarding symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 61(6), 564-576.
Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and attentional bias for emotional face.Psychological Science, 14(5), 409-415.
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1985). Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23(5), 563-569.
Mathews, A., & Klug, F. (1993). Emotionality and inteference with color-naming in anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31(1), 57-62.
Mattia, J. I., Heimberg, R. G., & Hope, D. A. (1993). The revised Stroop color-namingtask in social phobics. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31(3), 305-313.
McNally, R. J., Riemann, B. C., & Kim, E. (1990). Selective processing of threat cues in panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(5), 407-412.
Meyer, V., & Chesser, E. S. (1970). Behavior therapy in clinical psychiatry. Baltimore, MD: Penguin Books.
Mobini, S., & Grant, A. (2007). Clinical implications of attentional bias in anxiety disorders: An integrative literature review. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research,
Practice, Training, 44(4), 450-462.
Moritz, S., & von Muhlenen, A. (2008). Investigation of an attentional bias for fear-related material in obsessive-compulsive checkers. Depression and Anxiety,25(3), 225-229.
Mogg, K. & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Selective attention and anxiety: A cognitive-motivational perspective. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power (Eds.),Handbook of Cognition and Emotion. UK: John Wiley & Sons.
Mogg, K., Bradley, B., Dixon, C., Fisher, S., Twelftree, H., & McWilliams, A. (2000). Trait anxiety, defensiveness and selective processing of threat: An investigation using two measures of attentional bias.Personality and Individual Differences, 28(6), 1063-1077.
Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 29(1), 29-45.
Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Miles, F., & Dixon, R. (2004). Time course of attentional bias for threat scenes: Testing the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis. Cognition and Emotion, 18(5), 689-700.
Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2006). Time course of
attentional bias for fear-relevant pictures in spider-fearful individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(9),
1241-1250.
Mowrer, O. H. (1947). On the dual nature of learning: A reinterpretation of 'conditioning' and 'problem-solving.' Harvard Educational Review, 17, 102-148.
Mowrer, O. (1960). Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley.
Muller, J., & Roberts, J. E. (2005). Memory and attention in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A review. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19(1), 1-28.
Musa, C., Lepine, J. P., Clark, D. M., Mansell, W., & Ehlers, A. (2003). Selective attention in social phobia and the moderating effect of a concurrent depressive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy,41(9), 1043-1054.
Najmi, S., & Amir, N. (2010). The effect of attention training on a behavioral test of contamination fears in individuals with subclinical obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 119 (1), 136–142
Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group. (1997). Cognitive assessment ofobsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(7),667-681.
Öhman, A. (1993). Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: Clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives, and information processing mechanisms. In M. H. Lewins, & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions. New York: Guilford.
Omori, I. M., Murata, Y., Yamanishi, T., Nakaaki, S.,
Akechi, T., & Mikuni, M. (2007). The differential impact of executive attention dysfunction on episodic memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder patients with checking
symptoms vs. those with washing symptoms. Journal of
Psychiatric Research, 41(9), 776-784.
Ononaiye, M. S., Turpin, G., & Reidy, J. G. (2007).Attentional bias in social anxiety:Manipulation of stimulus duration and social-evaluative anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31(6), 727-740.
Pineles, S. L., Shipherd, J. C., Welch, L. P., &Yovel, I. (2007). The role of attentional biases in PTSD: Is it interference or facilitation? Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 45(8), 1903-1913.
Portman, M. E. (2009). Nature of generalized anxiety disorder. In M. E. Portman (Ed.), Generalized anxiety disorder across the lifespan: An integrative Approach (pp. 1-15). New York: Springer New York.
Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain.Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25-42.
Rachman, S., & Hodgson, R. (1980). Obsessions and compulsions. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
Rachman, S. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(9), 793-802.
Rachman, S. (1998). A cognitive theory of obsessions: Elaborations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(4), 385-401.
Rachman, S. (2002). A cognitive theory of compulsive checking. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(6), 624-639.
Rachman, S. (2004). Fear of contamination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(11), 1227-1255.
Radomsky, A. S., & Rachman, S. (1999). Memory bias in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(7), 605-618.
Rasmussen, S. A., & Tsuang, M. T. (1986). Clinical characteristics and family history in DSM-III obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 143,
317-322.
Rasmussen, S., & Eisen, J. (1998). The epidemiology and clinical features of obsessive compulsive disorder. In M. A. Jenike, L. Baer, & W. E. Minichiello (Eds.),Obsessive-compulsive disorders: Practical management ( pp. 12-43). St louis, MO: Mosby.
Reuther, E. T., Davis, T. E., Rudy, B. M., Jenkins, W. S., Whiting, S. E., & May, A.C. (2013). Intolerance of uncertainty as a mediator of the relationship between
perfectionism and obsessive-compulsive symptom severity. Depression and Anxiety, 30(8),773-777.
Rheaume, J., Freeston, M., Dugas, M. J., Letarte, H., & Ladouceur, R. (1995). Perfectionism, responsibility and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Behaviors. Research and Therapy, 33, 785-794.Rinck, M., Reinecke, A., Ellwart, T., Heuer, K., & Becker, E. S. (2005). Speeded detection and increased distraction in fear of spiders: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(2), 235-248.
Rohner, J.C. (2002). The time-course of visual threat processing: High trait anxious individuals eventually avert their gaze from angry faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16(6), 837-844.
Rusted, J., & Dighton, K. (1991). Selective processing of threat-related material by spiderphobics in a prose recall task. Cognition & Emotion, 5(2), 123-132.
Salemink, E., van den Hout, M. A., & Kindt, M. (2007). Selective attention and threat: Quick orienting versus slow disengagement and two versions of the dot probe task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(3), 607-615.
Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: A cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23(5), 571-583.
Salkovskis, P. M. (1989). Cognitive-behavioural factors and the persistence of intrusive thoughts in obsessional problems. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27(6),677-682.
Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Astin, J., & Freedman, B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(3), 373-386.
Solem, S., Myers, S. G., Fisher, P. L., Vogel, P. A., & Wells, A. (2010). An empirical test of the metacognitive model of obsessive-compulsive symptoms: Replication and extension. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(1), 79–86.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. (1970). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory ('Self-Evaluation Questionnaire'). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Sternberger, L. G., & Burns, G. L. (1990). Compulsive Activity Checklist and the Maudsley Obsessional-Compulsive Inventory: Psychometric properties of two measures of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behavior Therapy, 21(1), 117-127.
Storch, E., Roberti, J., & Roth, D. (2004). Factor structure, concurrent validity, and internal consistency of the Beck Depression Inventory in a sample of college
students. Depress Anxiety, 19(3), 187-189.
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interferences in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662.
Summerfeldt, L., Anotony, M. M., Downie, F., Richter, M. A., & Swinson, R. P. (1997). Prevalence of particualr obsessions and compulsions in a clinic sample. Unpublished manuscript. In Swinson, R. P., Antony, M.M., Rachman, S., &
Richter, M. A (Eds.), Obsessive-compulsive disorders: Theory, research and treatment (pp. 3-32). New York: Guilford.
Swinson, R. P., Anotony, M. M., Rachman, S., & Richter, M. A. (1998). Obsessive-compulsive disoeder : Theory,research, and treatment. New York: Guilford Press.
Tata, P. R., Leibowitz, J. A., Prunty, M. J., Cameron, M., & Pickering, A. D. (1996). Attentional bias in obsessional compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(1), 53-60.
Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Brigidi, B. D., Amir, N., Street, G. P., & Foa, E. B. (2001). Memory and memory confidence in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39(8), 913-927.
Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Brigidi, B. D., & Foa, E. B. (2003). Intolerance of uncertainty in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17,
233–242
Triesman, A. M., & Geffen, G. (1967). Selective attention: Perception or response? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19, 1-17.
Unoki, K., Kasuga, T., Matsushima, E., & Ohta, K. (1999). Attentional processing of emotional information in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry and Clinical
Neurosciences, 53, 635-642.
Wells, A., & Cartwright-Hatton, S. (2004). A short form of the metacognitions questionnaire: Properties of the MCQ-30. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(4), 385-396.
Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1994). Self-consciousness and cognitive failures as predictors of coping in stressful episodes. Cognition and Emotion, 8(3), 279-295.
Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1996). Modelling cognition in emotional disorder: The S-REF model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(11-12), 881-888.
Williams, J., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 3-24
Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C.,& Mathews, A. (1988). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.), Chichester, England: Wiley.
Wilson, K. A., & Chambless, D. L. (1999). Inflated perceptions of responsibility and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(4),
325-335.
Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(3), 665-681.
dc.identifier.urihttp://tdr.lib.ntu.edu.tw/jspui/handle/123456789/54809-
dc.description.abstract強迫性疾患(obsessive-compulsive disorders , OCD),又稱強迫症,主要有兩種核心症狀,分別為強迫思考(obsession)與強迫行為(compulsion)。本研究的目的在探討不同症狀類型的強迫性疾患的注意偏誤特性與歷程,並比較其與廣泛性焦慮疾患之差異。本研究之參與者來自北部某精神疾患專科醫院之門診病患與一般社區公民。前者主要由精神科醫師轉介具強迫性疾患與廣泛性焦慮疾患診斷之門診初診病患。本研究共計50位參與者,包括13位強迫檢查症狀患者,13位強迫清潔症狀患者,10位廣泛性焦慮疾患患者,以及14位一般社區公民。本研究以Koster(2004)提出的點偵測作業(dot-probe task)派典與注意偏誤指標(attentional bias index),探討注意偏誤的兩個主要機制,即注意警覺偏誤與注意轉移困難偏誤。以此二偏誤指標為依變項,以貝克憂鬱量表(BDI)之得分為共變項,進行4(組別:強迫症檢查症狀組、強迫症清潔症狀組、廣泛性焦慮疾患組、一般社區公民組) × 3(刺激字詞類別:強迫檢查字詞、強迫清潔字詞、正向情緒字詞)× 3(刺激呈現時間:100毫秒、500毫秒、1000毫秒)三因子混合設計之重複量數共變數分析。研究結果顯示,當面對威脅刺激時,強迫性疾患之注意偏誤歷程,在短刺激呈現時間會出現注意警覺偏誤現象,而後,隨刺激呈現時間增長,在中、長刺激呈現時間,其注意偏誤轉為注意轉移困難現象。此外,不同症狀類型之強迫性疾患在面對其核心症狀相關的威脅性刺激時,有近似的注意偏誤歷程,但對非核心症狀相關的刺激則有不同的注意歷程。另外,在面對正向刺激字詞時,亦有不同的注意偏誤歷程,僅強迫檢查症狀組對正向字詞在短刺激呈現時間裡,會有注意警覺偏誤現象,而在中、長刺激呈現時間則會有注意轉移困難偏誤現象;而強迫清潔症狀組則無論在短、中、長刺激呈現時間均不會出現注意警覺偏誤及注意轉移困難偏誤現象。此外,強迫檢查症狀組與廣泛性焦慮疾患組,對正向刺激字詞有近似的注意偏誤歷程。本研究的結果可用以說明不同類型之強迫性疾患,其核心症狀的同質性與異質性。本研究藉由比較不同類型之強迫性疾患,及其與廣泛性焦慮疾患間之注意偏誤歷程差異,據其結果可以擬定臨床之介入策略與治療方法。zh_TW
dc.description.abstractObsessive-compulsive disorder, also known as OCD, has two core symptoms, obsession and compulsion. The purpose of this study was to investigate the characteristics and course of attentional bias between different types of OCD, and compare the bias differences with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). Participants in this study came from a mental illness hospital outpatients and community citizens. The patients with OCD or GAD were referred by a psychiatrist. There were a total of 50 participants, including 13 OCD with checking symptoms, 13 OCD with cleaning symptoms, 10 GAD, and 14 community citizens. Koster’s (2004) dot-probe task design and attentional bias index were adopted to measure two major attention mechanisms of the attentional bias, namely, attention vigilance and attention disengagement difficulty. With these two bias indices as the dependent variables, and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) scores as the covariate, a 4 (groups: OCD with checking symptoms, OCD with cleaning symptoms, GAD, general community citizens) × 3 (stimulus word: checking words, cleaning words, positive emotion words) × 3 (stimulus exposure time: 100 ms, 500 ms, 1000 ms) mixed factorial design was conducted. The results showed that when faced with the threat stimuli, OCD patients revealed vigilant attention bias in the short stimulus exposure time, then, turned to attention disengagement difficulty in the middle and long stimulus exposure time. In addition, there was similar course of attentional bias in the two subtypes of OCD when they faced threats in accordance with their core symptom-related stimuli, but for the non-core symptom-related stimuli, they showed different attention process. Besides, in face of positive stimulus words, there was different attentional bias course between the two groups. Only OCD with checking symptoms revealed vigilant attention bias in the short exposure time, and difficulty disengagement in the medium and long stimulus exposure time, while OCD with cleaning symptoms did not show vigilant attention bias or attention disengagement difficulties in any exposure time. In addition, OCD with checking symptoms and GAD had similar attentional bias course for positive stimuli words. The results of this study can be used to illustrate the homogeneity and heterogeneity of the core symptoms in the different types of OCD. By comparing attention bias between types of OCD and GAD, the results of the present study suggest some possibilities to develop clinical intervention strategies and treatment methods.en
dc.description.provenanceMade available in DSpace on 2021-06-16T03:38:57Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1
ntu-104-R96227205-1.pdf: 2336651 bytes, checksum: 4a43edc5942502ed44a441e59b6f7080 (MD5)
Previous issue date: 2015
en
dc.description.tableofcontents第一章 緒論……………………………………………………………………1
第一節 強迫性疾患的定義與臨床特徵………………………………………1
第二節 強迫性疾患之共病現象………………………………………………7
第三節 強迫性疾患之異質性…………………………………………………9
第四節 強迫性疾患之心理病理機制 ………………………………………11
第五節 強迫性疾患之注意偏誤 ……………………………………………14
第六節 研究目的與假設 ……………………………………………………32
第二章 研究方法 ……………………………………………………………37
第一節 參與者 ………………………………………………………………37
第二節 實驗設計 ……………………………………………………………39
第三節 自我陳述量表 ………………………………………………………39
第四節 點偵測作業實驗材料(前置作業) ………………………………42
第五節 點偵測作業 …………………………………………………………45
第六節 實驗程序 ……………………………………………………………46
第七節 資料分析 ……………………………………………………………47
第三章 研究結果 ……………………………………………………………51
第一節 參與者性別、教育、年齡與自陳量表得分 ………………………51
第二節 注意偏誤指標的分析 ………………………………………………58
第三節 注意警覺指標的分析 ………………………………………………74
第四節 注意轉移困難指標的分析 …………………………………………95
第五節 注意偏誤指標分析小結……………………………………………117
第四章 討論…………………………………………………………………127
第一節 注意偏誤現象………………………………………………………127
第二節 注意偏誤現象之病理機制探討……………………………………135
第三節 研究貢獻與臨床運用………………………………………………139
第四節 研究限制……………………………………………………………142
第五節 建議與未來發展方向………………………………………………146
參考文獻 ……………………………………………………………………149
附錄 …………………………………………………………………………165
附錄一 點偵測作業練習階段之指導語內容………………………………165
附錄二 點偵測作業正式實驗階段之指導語內容…………………………165
附錄三 不刺激材料字詞詞頻數、筆畫數、情緒值之平均數與標準差…166
附錄四 詞頻變異數分析結果摘要表………………………………………166
附錄五 筆畫數變異數分析結果摘要表……………………………………166
附錄六 情緒值變異數分析結果摘要表……………………………………167
dc.language.isozh-TW
dc.title強迫性疾患之注意偏誤特性:時間歷程分析zh_TW
dc.titleAttentional Bias in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder : A Time Course Analysisen
dc.typeThesis
dc.date.schoolyear103-1
dc.description.degree碩士
dc.contributor.oralexamcommittee卓淑玲,鄧閔鴻
dc.subject.keyword強迫性疾患,注意偏誤,點偵測作業,注意警覺指標,注意轉移困難,zh_TW
dc.subject.keywordobsessive-compulsive disorders,attention bias,dot-probe task,attention vigilance index,attention disengagement difficulty index,en
dc.relation.page167
dc.rights.note有償授權
dc.date.accepted2015-02-25
dc.contributor.author-college理學院zh_TW
dc.contributor.author-dept心理學研究所zh_TW
顯示於系所單位:心理學系

文件中的檔案:
檔案 大小格式 
ntu-104-1.pdf
  目前未授權公開取用
2.28 MBAdobe PDF
顯示文件簡單紀錄


系統中的文件,除了特別指名其著作權條款之外,均受到著作權保護,並且保留所有的權利。

社群連結
聯絡資訊
10617臺北市大安區羅斯福路四段1號
No.1 Sec.4, Roosevelt Rd., Taipei, Taiwan, R.O.C. 106
Tel: (02)33662353
Email: ntuetds@ntu.edu.tw
意見箱
相關連結
館藏目錄
國內圖書館整合查詢 MetaCat
臺大學術典藏 NTU Scholars
臺大圖書館數位典藏館
本站聲明
© NTU Library All Rights Reserved